20
views
0
recommends
+1 Recommend
0 collections
    0
    shares
      • Record: found
      • Abstract: found
      • Article: not found

      Keeping the goal in mind: Prefrontal contributions to spatial navigation

      research-article
      *
      Neuropsychologia
      Pergamon Press
      Wayfinding, Spatial memory, Inhibition, Planning, Orbitofrontal

      Read this article at

      ScienceOpenPublisherPMC
      Bookmark
          There is no author summary for this article yet. Authors can add summaries to their articles on ScienceOpen to make them more accessible to a non-specialist audience.

          Abstract

          On his voyage home, Odysseus is nearly seduced to his death by the singing of the sirens. Odysseus's crew, their ears plugged with wax, thankfully do not hear his desperate pleas to change course. While such trials are not a feature of our daily travels, a variety of distractions can send us off course. In this issue, Ciaramelli (2008) reports a patient who repeatedly suffers from this affliction, being lured away to unintended locations. The study provides new insights into the neural basis of spatial navigation. Our capacity for navigation is thought to rely on a distributed network of brain regions, which include the hippocampus, parahippocampus, retrosplenial cortex, parietal cortex, and caudate nucleus (see e.g. Aguirre & D’Esposito, 1999; Spiers & Maguire, 2007a). Each region is thought to serve a different function of our navigation machinery, such as representing a map of large-scale space, converting the map to egocentric space, or representing our current viewpoint. However, the neural substrate of one particular function remains a mystery. How does the brain represent spatial goal locations or guide navigation to them? Several lines of evidence suggest this may be the preserve of the prefrontal cortex. The prefrontal cortex has long been associated with behavioural flexibility, working memory and planning; functions important for achieving goals (Fuster, 1989; Luria, 1969; Passingham, 1993). It also integrates highly processed information important for guiding goal-directed behaviour (Pandya & Barnes, 1987). Several functional neuroimaging studies have revealed increased activity in prefrontal areas during spatial navigation tasks (Gron, Wunderlich, Spitzer, Tomczak, & Riepe, 2000; Hartley, Maguire, Spiers, & Burgess, 2003; Maguire et al., 1998; Yoshida & Ishii, 2006), one directly linking prefrontal activity to goal processing (Spiers & Maguire, 2006, 2007b). In rodents, medial and orbital prefrontal lesions have been found to impair certain aspects of navigation (Lacroix, White, & Feldon, 2002; Vafaei & Rashidy-Pour, 2004), and recently cells in these regions have been found to code spatial information about goals (Feierstein, Quirk, Uchida, Sosulski, & Mainen, 2006; Hok, Save, Lenck-Santini, & Poucet, 2005). However, it remains uncertain whether the human prefrontal cortex is necessary for navigation. In a recent article in Neuropsychologia new evidence has emerged suggesting that the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) is required for navigation (Ciaramelli, 2008). Ciaramelli (2008) tested a patient with bilateral damage to the ventromedial prefrontal and rostral anterior cingulate cortices whose central complaint, following recovery, was of way-finding difficulties. Despite good topographical knowledge of his hometown he performed very poorly, compared to healthy controls, when asked to describe a set of routes between locations in the town. However, his performance improved substantially when he was given the name of his destination or a cue to rehearse the destination at regular intervals. No such improvement occurred when a visual stimulus was presented at similar intervals. Thus, it appears the vmPFC is necessary for navigation and its role may be to maintain the goal destination in working memory (Ciaramelli, 2008). That the problem lies with working memory is further supported by the patient's generalised deficits on standardized tasks requiring working memory. However, deficits elicited by laboratory tasks do not always predict deficits in the ‘real world’ (Habib & Sirigu, 1987; Kapur & Pearson, 1983; Maguire, Burke, Phillips, & Staunton, 1996; Spiers, Burgess, Hartley, Vargha-Khadem, & O’Keefe, 2001). Indeed, a cue to rehearse the destination might not be as beneficial during navigation of a complex and bustling town. Appropriately, the patient's ability to actively navigate in the town was assessed under similar cue conditions to the laboratory task, with a strikingly similar pattern of results. Further insight into this patient's problem was generated by an analysis of the errors made in relation to several factors, including familiarity, route length, number of turns, etc. (Ciaramelli, 2008). Of these factors, only familiarity was found to be associated with the number of errors. While successful routes were rated more familiar, surprisingly, the reverse was true of highly familiar locations on the route. Intriguingly, two-thirds of error trials involved the route ending at one of a number of personally familiar locations, each associated with the patient's previous work or hobbies. According to Ciaramelli (2008) these locations, being highly salient, acted as “attractor” locations, luring the patient away from his true goal. Thus, the vmPFC may be necessary not only to maintain the goal in memory, but also to suppress irrelevant information. These findings agree well with those of a recent neuroimaging study exploring the brain activity of London taxi drivers as they navigated a highly accurate virtual simulation of London (UK) (Spiers & Maguire, 2006, 2007b). In this study, the relationship between the subject's thoughts during navigation and their brain activity was examined using a retrospective verbal report protocol. Immediately post-scan, subjects watched a video replay of their performance and reported what they had been thinking while they were doing the task in the scanner. Of the wide variety of thoughts reported, the most frequent concerned thinking about the goal and the route to it (see Spiers & Maguire, in press, for full details). These were associated with increased activity in anterior BA10 and in a medial prefrontal region overlapping with the dorsal extent of the lesion of the patient described by Ciaramelli (2008). Given the latest findings, it could be argued the medial region is involved in maintaining goal representation while B10 may be important for manipulating information for planning (Koechlin, Basso, Pietrini, Panzer, & Grafman, 1999). Further evidence that cells in the medial prefrontal region might monitor spatial goals comes from the finding that activity in a region on the border of BA9 and BA32 correlated with proximity to the goal during navigation (Spiers & Maguire, 2007b). A more ventral medial prefrontal region was activated when subjects listened to navigationally irrelevant comments of customers whilst navigating (Spiers & Maguire, 2006). Ciaramelli (2008) suggests this may have been related to the mental rehearsal or ‘energizing’ of the actual goals. An alternative, related, interpretation is that the region is activated by the requirement to suppress processing of the new information in posterior cortical regions. Ciaramelli (2008) argues that the patient's deficit may arise from three related difficulties. These are: (a) an inability to actively maintain the goal in working memory, (b) a reversal-learning problem, and (c) a loss of the grammar that prescribes the transitions between attractor states in cortical networks. The last problem would make it difficult for the patient to avoid personally familiar locations, as these would be represented by strong attractors. A view favoured here, in line with other authors (Burgess, Veitch, Costello, & Shallice, 2000; Shallice & Burgess, 1991), is that the deficit can more readily be summarised as a failure to maintain the intention to reach the destination in working memory and a reduced suppression of previously learned information (in this case routes). A similar lack of suppression may underlie the reversal-learning deficit seen in such patients (Fellows & Farah, 2003), rather than vice versa. Most new research generates more questions than answers, as is the case here. Along with replicating the findings in other similar patients, a number of questions remain to be answered. Can the patient learn routes in an unfamiliar environment? At what point in the route is the intention lost? Given that medial prefrontal activity is correlated with goal proximity (Spiers & Maguire, 2007b), might the patient be unable to make use of novel shortcuts or reach the goal after a detour? Which region calculates proximity or directional information to goals? Though there is much still to be explored, the results from Ciaramelli (2008) add to our understanding of the neural processes supporting navigation. Given this new insight, when your mind next drifts and you arrive at a familiar but unintended destination, you will know which part of your neuroanatomy to blame.

          Related collections

          Most cited references23

          • Record: found
          • Abstract: found
          • Article: not found

          Knowing where and getting there: a human navigation network.

          The neural basis of navigation by humans was investigated with functional neuroimaging of brain activity during navigation in a familiar, yet complex virtual reality town. Activation of the right hippocampus was strongly associated with knowing accurately where places were located and navigating accurately between them. Getting to those places quickly was strongly associated with activation of the right caudate nucleus. These two right-side brain structures function in the context of associated activity in right inferior parietal and bilateral medial parietal regions that support egocentric movement through the virtual town, and activity in other left-side regions (hippocampus, frontal cortex) probably involved in nonspatial aspects of navigation. These findings outline a network of brain areas that support navigation in humans and link the functions of these regions to physiological observations in other mammals.
            Bookmark
            • Record: found
            • Abstract: found
            • Article: not found

            The role of the anterior prefrontal cortex in human cognition.

            Complex problem-solving and planning involve the most anterior part of the frontal lobes including the fronto-polar prefrontal cortex (FPPC), which is especially well developed in humans compared with other primates. The specific role of this region in human cognition, however, is poorly understood. Here we show, using functional magnetic resonance imaging, that bilateral regions in the FPPC alone are selectively activated when subjects have to keep in mind a main goal while performing concurrent (sub)goals. Neither keeping in mind a goal over time (working memory) nor successively allocating attentional resources between alternative goals (dual-task performance) could by themselves activate these regions. Our results indicate that the FPPC selectively mediates the human ability to hold in mind goals while exploring and processing secondary goals, a process generally required in planning and reasoning.
              Bookmark
              • Record: found
              • Abstract: found
              • Article: not found

              Topographical disorientation: a synthesis and taxonomy.

              Over the last century, several dozen case reports have presented 'topographically disoriented' patients who, in some cases, appear to have selectively lost their ability to find their way within large-scale, locomotor environments. A review is offered here that has as its aim the creation of a taxonomy that accurately reflects the behavioural impairments and neuroanatomical findings of this literature. This effort is guided by an appreciation of the models of normative way-finding offered by environmental psychology and recent neuroscience research. It is proposed that several varieties of topographical disorientation exist, resulting from damage to distinct neuroanatomical areas. The particular pattern of impairments that patients evidence is argued to be consonant with the known functions of these cortical regions and with recent neuroimaging results. The conflicting claims of previous reviews of this area are also considered and addressed.
                Bookmark

                Author and article information

                Journal
                Neuropsychologia
                Neuropsychologia
                Pergamon Press
                0028-3932
                1873-3514
                June 2008
                June 2008
                : 46
                : 7
                : 2106-2108
                Affiliations
                Institute of Behavioural Neuroscience, Department of Psychology, University College London, 26 Bedford Way, London WC1H 0AP, United Kingdom
                Author notes
                [* ]Tel.: +44 7670 7553. h.spiers@ 123456ucl.ac.uk
                Article
                NSY2838
                10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.01.028
                2430985
                18387640
                01b36a42-0b65-4e38-afe4-b38d83c0d510
                © 2008 Elsevier Ltd.

                This document may be redistributed and reused, subject to certain conditions.

                History
                Categories
                Article

                Neurology
                wayfinding,spatial memory,orbitofrontal,inhibition,planning
                Neurology
                wayfinding, spatial memory, orbitofrontal, inhibition, planning

                Comments

                Comment on this article