13
views
0
recommends
+1 Recommend
0 collections
    0
    shares
      • Record: found
      • Abstract: found
      • Article: found
      Is Open Access

      Concordance on zebra stripes: a comment on Larison et al. (2015)

      article-commentary
      1 , 2
      Royal Society Open Science
      The Royal Society Publishing

      Read this article at

      Bookmark
          There is no author summary for this article yet. Authors can add summaries to their articles on ScienceOpen to make them more accessible to a non-specialist audience.

          Abstract

          The functional significance of the extraordinary black and white stripes of zebras is still mysterious but now an active field of research. Four major hypotheses have been put forward: stripes are an antipredator defence operating through crypsis [1] or confusion of predators [2], are a means of reinforcing social bonds [3], are defence against ectoparasites [4] or are a means of cooling zebras [5]. Now, in the second multifactorial analysis of striping in zebras to date, Larison et al. [6] investigated the environmental factors that explain geographical variation in striping within a single species, the plains zebra (Equus quagga or Equus burchellii). They matched variation in striping patterns at 16 sites across its geographical range to a suite of environmental variables as well as tsetse fly (glossinid) distribution and lion (Panthera leo) presence. They found that greater intensities of intraspecific striping were associated with warmer temperatures and high precipitation. Last year we published a similar but phylogenetically controlled analysis using all seven species of equids, namely the three striped zebra species, the African wild ass (Equus africanus) which has leg stripes but no body stripes, and the unstriped species of equids (E. hemionus, E. kiang, E. ferus przewalski), and 20 of their subspecies, and compared several aspects of the four hypotheses [7]. These were camouflage in woodlands, antipredator defence against both lions and spotted hyaenas (Crocuta crocuta), social interactions, tsetse fly distribution, tabanid distribution using both temperature and humidity ranges as a proxy for abundance, and temperature isoclines to assess the cooling hypothesis. First we ran univariate tests on individual factors to identify the hypotheses with the strongest predictive ability, then subsequently used AICc model selection procedures to pit the most promising predictors against each other—a powerful statistical protocol for testing multiple hypotheses. Further, our study was corrected for phylogenetic relatedness between the species and subspecies, allowing us to draw conclusions about correlated evolution between body striping and environmental selective pressures. Our study provided strong evidence that our proxy for tabanid fly parasitism was essentially perfectly correlated with the presence of stripes across species across almost every area of the body, and especially for leg stripes, the level at which biting flies prefer to land on hosts. Our comparative study pertains to the factors that maintain striping across species of equids, whereas Larison and co-workers' conclusions are specific to the ecocorrelates of variation in striping within plains zebras, and cannot necessarily be extrapolated directly to infer the selective pressures that favoured the original evolution of zebra stripes, which only occurred once in the genus. That said, these two studies, conducted independently of each other, have now tested a number of hypotheses for the function of striping in zebras, with both studies using a multifactorial approach that pits the hypotheses against each other allowing for direct tests of multiple hypotheses within the same model. Here we want to stress that these two studies agree on a number of points that move detective work on the mystery of zebra stripes forward considerably. With regard to individual hypotheses, both studies found no convincing evidence for the geographical distribution of stripes on any part of the body being associated with lion distribution across the African continent. Both studies remark that this is unsurprising because a considerable body of literature finds that zebras are a preferred prey of lions in most ecosystems [8]. Taken together, our findings therefore cast serious doubt on whether experiments involving human subjects capturing striped objects moving across computer screens (analogies for lions chasing zebras) are worthwhile tests of the adaptive significance of striping in zebras. Second, our studies found no evidence for striping being associated with woodland or tree cover, refuting the idea that stripes are a form of crypsis against predators. Third, neither study found strong evidence for variation in striping being related to glossinid (tsetse) distributions. The only significant association was between belly stripe number and tsetse flies in Caro et al. [7]; none was found for other parts of the body in either analysis. The studies additionally concur in finding aspects of striping being related to temperature and moisture. We found many aspects of striping (number of face stripes, neck stripes, flank striping, rump striping, shadow stripe severity and leg stripe intensity) are associated with temperatures lying between 15°C and 30°C and humidity between 30 and 85% (a proxy for tabanid abundance) when these conditions persist for a minimum of six to seven months. Note, such warm humid conditions are congruent with Larison et al.'s precipitation during the wettest month of the year (BIO13 in WorldClim) and constant annual temperatures (BIO3) which they found were associated with striping measures in plains zebras. In short, the studies find that striping is associated with warm humid conditions both inter- and intraspecifically. Where the studies differ is in their interpretation of these results. Larison and co-workers focus on their temperature rather than on their rainfall association and discuss how temperature is correlated with striping measures in plains zebras although they are agnostic as to mechanism. While acknowledging that stripes might set up convection currents over the torso [5] and so cool the animal, in regards to the fore and hindlegs where they found positive associations with temperature too, they write ‘The association between temperature and striping on the legs is not easily explained as a mechanism for thermoregulation. It may simply be a result of genetic correlation as stripe characteristics on the legs and torso are highly correlated, or it may be a response to a different mechanism, such as avoiding biting flies’. We would suggest that as leg stripes but not body stripes are found in non-zebra species such as E. africanus that are under heavy tabanid pressure, body stripes might stem from genetic correlation with leg stripes since more species have leg stripes than body stripes. Caro and others suggest that striping is associated with tabanid and possibly other biting fly annoyance. There is experimental evidence that tabanids eschew landing on white [9], striped (e.g. [4,10]) and spotted surfaces [11], that zebra stripe widths are thinner than those on which horse flies, stable flies and tsetse flies prefer to land, that zebra pelage is thinner than the length of biting fly mouthparts making them susceptible to blood loss, and because several groups of biting flies carry diseases fatal to zebras [7]. By contrast, there is no experimental evidence as yet for or against the setting up of convection currents by stripe patterns over the zebra dorsum or other body areas. Moreover, against cooling being the primary selective pressure driving black- and white-striped pelage, we would add that thick saturated black stripes that absorb radiation more than thinner, less saturated black stripes or white stripes would be unlikely to be found in hotter climates if there is a premium on staying cool; that any shadow stripes lying between deep black stripes would disrupt reflective properties of white stripes; and that convection currents would be ineffective on windy floodplains often inhabited by plains zebras or when animals are moving. Indeed, like artiodactyls [12], we would expect other ungulates to wear entirely light-coloured coats of high albedo in hotter environments. Finally, measurements of free-living plains zebras and sympatric ungulates using an infrared camera show that zebras are always significantly warmer than three sympatric herbivores photographed under the same conditions: mean flank temperatures, zebra 36.0°C (N=57 individuals), impalas Aepyceros melampus 34.2°C (N=49), buffalo Syncerus caffer 34.1°C (N=35) and giraffe Giraffa camelopardalis 33.3°C (N=27) [13]. Interestingly, Larison and her co-workers suggest that trypanosomes and other diseases carried by tsetse flies may have a compromised ability to develop at colder temperatures, reminding readers that the distribution of biting flies may have less relevance than the distribution of diseases carried by those flies. They concede that their temperature-striping associations could reflect favourable conditions for infectious diseases carried by biting flies that adversely affect zebras: ‘We suggest that temperature may influence trypanosome prevalence in tsetse flies and as a consequence help explain variation in striping’. Caro and others tested an additional hypothesis interspecifically that the Larison group did not test intraspecifically. This was striping being associated with mean and maximum group sizes as a proxy for social interactions, for which we found no evidence. While it is often expedient to call attention to differences in findings between studies, in the interests of publication and publicity, it is often more constructive in the long run to focus on similarities. Here, we suggest that the only two multifactorial studies of this problem (table 1), conducted independently, have remarkable concordance in (i) dismissing lion predation as driving striping in equids, (ii) rejecting stripes having a cryptic function in wooded habitats, (iii) casting aspersions on tsetse fly distributions being of major importance for striping, and (iv) either questioning [6] or dismissing [7] the idea that stripes can generate cooling eddies, respectively, on the legs or anywhere on the body, and (v) indirectly point to tabanid or other biting fly (e.g. muscid, simuliid or mosquito) annoyance being the evolutionary driver of striping in Equidae, most probably because of the diseases that they carry and for which they are renowned [13]. Table 1. Summary of findings from the two multifactorial studies of striping in zebras. hypothesis comparative test: Caro et al. [7] intraspecific test: Larison et al. [6] notes predation crypsis no no no association with trees spotted hyaena unlikely not tested lion no no zebras are preferred prey social interaction no not tested all equids highly social antiparasite glossinid no (belly yes) no carry diseases fatal to equids and do not like to land on stripes tabanid yes not tested carry diseases fatal to equids and do not like to land on stripes cooling no yes mechanism unclear: includes both temperature regulation and disease The studies' joint handicap, as both sets of authors explicitly recognize, is that we are hampered by lack of understanding of the extent to which temperature and humidity predict fly annoyance, the specific problems posed by biting flies, the extent to which biting fly distributions and the diseases that they can carry elide, and the mechanism by which stripes prevent flies from landing on zebra pelage mediated through disruption of outline, modulation of brightness or of polarized light [4], or confusion of insect motion detection systems controlling their approach and landing [14]. We suggest that future research targeted at these issues will improve our understanding of why zebras have black and white stripes.

          Related collections

          Most cited references5

          • Record: found
          • Abstract: found
          • Article: not found

          Polarotactic tabanids find striped patterns with brightness and/or polarization modulation least attractive: an advantage of zebra stripes.

          The characteristic striped appearance of zebras has provoked much speculation about its function and why the pattern has evolved, but experimental evidence is scarce. Here, we demonstrate that a zebra-striped horse model attracts far fewer horseflies (tabanids) than either homogeneous black, brown, grey or white equivalents. Such biting flies are prevalent across Africa and have considerable fitness impact on potential mammalian hosts. Besides brightness, one of the likely mechanisms underlying this protection is the polarization of reflected light from the host animal. We show that the attractiveness of striped patterns to tabanids is also reduced if only polarization modulations (parallel stripes with alternating orthogonal directions of polarization) occur in horizontal or vertical homogeneous grey surfaces. Tabanids have been shown to respond strongly to linearly polarized light, and we demonstrate here that the light and dark stripes of a zebra's coat reflect very different polarizations of light in a way that disrupts the attractiveness to tabanids. We show that the attractiveness to tabanids decreases with decreasing stripe width, and that stripes below a certain size are effective in not attracting tabanids. Further, we demonstrate that the stripe widths of zebra coats fall in a range where the striped pattern is most disruptive to tabanids. The striped coat patterns of several other large mammals may also function in reducing exposure to tabanids by similar mechanisms of differential brightness and polarization of reflected light. This work provides an experimentally supported explanation for the underlying mechanism leading to the selective advantage of a black-and-white striped coat pattern.
            Bookmark
            • Record: found
            • Abstract: found
            • Article: not found

            Motion camouflage induced by zebra stripes.

            The functional significance of the zebra coat stripe pattern is one of the oldest questions in evolutionary biology, having troubled scientists ever since Charles Darwin and Alfred Russel Wallace first disagreed on the subject. While different theories have been put forward to address this question, the idea that the stripes act to confuse or 'dazzle' observers remains one of the most plausible. However, the specific mechanisms by which this may operate have not been investigated in detail. In this paper, we investigate how motion of the zebra's high contrast stripes creates visual effects that may act as a form of motion camouflage. We simulated a biologically motivated motion detection algorithm to analyse motion signals generated by different areas on a zebra's body during displacements of their retinal images. Our simulations demonstrate that the motion signals that these coat patterns generate could be a highly misleading source of information. We suggest that the observer's visual system is flooded with erroneous motion signals that correspond to two well-known visual illusions: (i) the wagon-wheel effect (perceived motion inversion due to spatiotemporal aliasing); and (ii) the barber-pole illusion (misperceived direction of motion due to the aperture effect), and predict that these two illusory effects act together to confuse biting insects approaching from the air, or possibly mammalian predators during the hunt, particularly when two or more zebras are observed moving together as a herd.
              Bookmark
              • Record: found
              • Abstract: found
              • Article: found
              Is Open Access

              How the zebra got its stripes: a problem with too many solutions

              The adaptive significance of zebra stripes has thus far eluded understanding. Many explanations have been suggested, including social cohesion, thermoregulation, predation evasion and avoidance of biting flies. Identifying the associations between phenotypic and environmental factors is essential for testing these hypotheses and substantiating existing experimental evidence. Plains zebra striping pattern varies regionally, from heavy black and white striping over the entire body in some areas to reduced stripe coverage with thinner and lighter stripes in others. We examined how well 29 environmental variables predict the variation in stripe characteristics of plains zebra across their range in Africa. In contrast to recent findings, we found no evidence that striping may have evolved to escape predators or avoid biting flies. Instead, we found that temperature successfully predicts a substantial amount of the stripe pattern variation observed in plains zebra. As this association between striping and temperature may be indicative of multiple biological processes, we suggest that the selective agents driving zebra striping are probably multifarious and complex.
                Bookmark

                Author and article information

                Journal
                R Soc Open Sci
                R Soc Open Sci
                RSOS
                royopensci
                Royal Society Open Science
                The Royal Society Publishing
                2054-5703
                September 2015
                30 September 2015
                30 September 2015
                : 2
                : 9
                : 150323
                Affiliations
                [1 ]Department of Wildlife, Fish and Conservation Biology, University of California , Davis, CA 95616, USA
                [2 ]Department of Biological Sciences, California State University , Long Beach, CA 90840, USA
                Author notes
                Author for correspondence: Tim Caro e-mail: tmcaro@ 123456ucdavis.edu
                Article
                rsos150323
                10.1098/rsos.150323
                4593687
                422f06f3-1ccb-414c-aec6-edbb7ac7de81

                © 2015 The Authors. Published by the Royal Society under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/, which permits unrestricted use, provided the original author and source are credited.

                History
                : 16 February 2015
                : 7 July 2015
                Categories
                1001
                70
                Biology (Whole Organism)
                Comment
                Custom metadata
                September, 2015

                Comments

                Comment on this article