The outbreak of the novel coronavirus, 2019-nCoV, has dominated headlines throughout
the world. This is not surprising. The virus is new, rapidly spreading, with a mortality
rate of about 2% at the time of writing this editorial, and there are many uncertainties
concerning its origins, nature, and course. The number of 2019-nCoV infections continue
to rise, as do the number of deaths. People are being quarantined. Surgical masks
and gloves, often used as a barrier to viral transmission, are selling out, even though
health authorities such as the World Health Organization (WHO) and the Centers for
Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) advise people that masks and gloves are not useful
or necessary for avoiding infection in healthy people.
Data from recent public opinion polls show that 2019-nCoV is having a significant
psychological impact. An Angus Reid poll of 1354 Canadian adults conducted in early
February 2020 indicated that one-third of respondents were worried about the virus
and 7% were “very concerned” about becoming infected (Angus Reid Institute, 2020).
At the time of the poll, only 4 Canadians were infected, indicating a very low risk
for a country of approximately 37 million; yet, 7% of the population—that is, 2.6
million people—were very concerned. There was also an increase in hygienic and avoidance
behaviours. Since hearing about the outbreak, 3% of respondents in the Angus Reid
poll had purchased a facemask, 41% washed their hands more often, 4% avoided taking
public transit, and 12% avoided public places (Angus Reid Institute, 2020). Consistent
with findings from previous epidemics and pandemics (Taylor, 2019), many people in
this poll (33%) were not confident that the healthcare system in their community was
prepared to deal with new cases of the coronavirus. Lack of faith in the healthcare
system is likely to fuel fears about the consequences of becoming infected.
Broadly similar findings were reported in U.S. polls. A poll of 808 U.S. adults was
conducted on January 31-February 1, 2020 (National Public Radio, 2020). Most respondents
(66%) thought that 2019-nCoV was a real threat and most (56%) were very concerned
about the spread of the coronavirus in the U.S. About a quarter (26%) of respondents
thought the U.S. government was not doing enough to prevent the spread of infection.
The Morning Consult company conducted a poll of 2200 American adults from January
24–26, 2020, at which time there were 5 cases of the coronavirus in the U.S. and no
fatalities (Morning Consult, 2020). Over a third of respondents (37%) said they were
very concerned about the coronavirus spreading in the U.S. A quarter (25%) of respondents
were more worried about the coronavirus than they were about the 2014 outbreak of
Ebola virus disease. Many respondents were not confident that the spread of infection
could be controlled by the CDC or by President Donald Trump (20% and 54%, respectively).
In the wake of the 2019-nCoV outbreak, there have been numerous reports of xenophobia
directed toward Chinese people (Aguilera, 2020). For example, there have been reports
of Chinese restaurants having to close or lay off staff because patrons are avoiding
the premises, Chinese nationals have been barred from some restaurants, and cruise
ship operators have announced bans on Chinese nationals from traveling on their cruise
ships (e.g., Evelyn, 2020; Lowen, 2020). Many (32%) U.S. respondents in the Morning
Consult poll blamed the Chinese government for the spread of 2019-nCoV into the U.S.
The rise of infection-related xenophobia has been reported in many previous epidemics
and pandemics, and appears to be an unfortunately common response when people are
threatened with an infection that originates from outside of their community (Taylor,
2019).
The fear of 2019-nCoV is likely due to its novelty and the uncertainties about how
bad the current outbreak might become. Fear of 2019-nCoV is much greater than fear
of seasonal influenza, even though the latter has killed considerably more people.
According to the Morning Consult (2020) poll, 37% of Americans were very concerned
about 2019-nCoV whereas 27% were very concerned about seasonal influenza, and most
respondents (62%) were more worried about 2019-nCoV than they were about seasonal
influenza. These figures contrast sharply with the actual number of infections and
deaths in the U.S. due to these viruses. As of February 8, 2020, there had been 11
cases of 2019-nCoV in the U.S. and no fatalities. Yet during the seasonal flu season
in the U.S. (i.e., from October 2019 to February 2020) there had been 22 million flu
illnesses, 210,000 hospitalizations because of influenza, and 12,000 flu-related deaths
(CDC, 2020). In other words, the American public was more worried about 11 cases of
2019-nCoV with no fatalities than it was about 22 million cases of flu infection and
12,000 fatalities.
While the nature and impact of 2019-nCoV on mental health remains to be determined,
there are clues in the existing literature that may help us begin to understand what
to expect. Research on the psychological reactions to previous epidemics and pandemics
suggests that various psychological vulnerability factors may play a role in coronaphobia,
including individual difference variables such as the intolerance of uncertainty,
perceived vulnerability to disease, and anxiety (worry) proneness (Taylor, 2019).
More research is needed to understand the relationship between coronaphobia and coronavirus-related
xenophobia. Research from other outbreaks of infectious disease suggests that individual
difference variables such as the perceived vulnerability to disease may play a role
in both coronaphobia and coronavirus-related xenophobia (Taylor, 2019). Likewise,
lack of information and misinformation, often aided by sensational popular media headlines
and foci, have been shown to fuel health-related fears and phobias (Taylor & Asmundson,
2004). These factors may also play a significant role in coronophobia.
Much also remains to be learned about the best ways of reducing coronaphobia and related
xenophobia. If infection is widespread, these phobias also will likely be widespread.
Screen-and-treat approaches for coronaphobia could be implemented, in conjunction
with community-based interventions for both infection-related fears and xenophobia
(e.g., educational materials) (Taylor, 2019). The merits of such interventions, as
well as the most accessible forms of delivery, remain to be investigated.
An important question is whether healthcare systems throughout the world are ready
to deal with the surge of so-called “worried well” patients; that is, the surge into
hospital emergency rooms of people who misinterpret their bodily sensations as signs
of potential infection with the 2019-nCoV coronavirus. During the 2009 H1N1 influenza
pandemic there were reports of hospitals being flooded with “worried well” patients
who mistakenly believed that their benign coughs or fevers were indications of pandemic
influenza (Taylor, 2019). The same will occur for 2019-nCoV, and is likely happening
right now in Wuhan, China, as we write this editorial.
A great deal of media attention has been devoted to the critical question of whether
the healthcare systems throughout the world are ready to deal with the influx of cases
of coronavirus infection. What has been lacking in the media, and in news briefs from
the WHO and CDC, is a discussion of whether we are ready for a surge of patients into
hospital emergency rooms whose problems are not coronavirus, but minor respiratory
ailments combined with coronaphobia. The current outbreak of 2019-nCoV represents
a call to action for psychosocial researchers and practitioners. It is vitally important
to understand the psychosocial fallout of 2019-nCoV, such as excessive fear (or lack
of concern and due caution) and discrimination, and to find evidence-based ways of
addressing these issues. This will be important not only for 2019-nCoV, but also for
future outbreaks of infection. Regardless of whether 2019-nCoV becomes a pandemic,
virologists predict that the next severe pandemic is inevitable and may arrive in
the coming years (Taylor, 2019).