Risk factors for breast cancer are often confusing and contradictory. Discrepancies
are likely due to different subtypes having divergent risk factors. An important distinction
between breast cancer subtypes is hormone-receptor status. Compared to women diagnosed
with estrogen receptor positive (ER+) breast cancer, those with estrogen receptor
negative (ER−) tumors are usually diagnosed at a younger age and have a higher mortality
[1]. Few studies have attempted to explain ‘why’ breast cancer subtypes have different
risk factors.
In a recent meta-analysis, Aktipis et al. [2] demonstrated that modern reproductive
behaviors are more strongly associated with the development of ER+ than ER− breast
cancer. The systematic review specifically reported the following: (a) fewer total
offspring is a risk factor for ER+, but not ER−, tumors; (b) older age at first birth
is associated with risk for ER+, but not ER−, tumors and (c) lower age of menarche
is a risk factor for both ER+ and ER− tumors. These results support the evolutionary
mismatch hypothesis for ER+ breast cancer susceptibility; that is, modern women have
a higher risk from more menstrual cycles and greater cumulative exposure to estrogen
compared to ancestral humans. However, modern reproductive patterns seem to have little
influence on hormone-independent breast cancer risk. Breast cancer susceptibility
may require complementary evolutionary explanations. In this article, we propose that
the risk factors for ER− breast cancer, low socioeconomic status (SES), poor diet
and early age of menarche are features of a faster life history strategy.
Life history theory provides a framework for understanding how, when and why organisms
allocate their resources [3]. To maximize reproductive success, organisms must strategically
distribute resources toward growth, reproduction and somatic maintenance. This process
of phenotypic development is largely determined by trade-offs. Three of the most important
trade-offs include reproduction versus survival (i.e. growth and somatic maintenance),
offspring now versus offspring later and offspring quality versus offspring quantity
(Fig. 1).
Figure 1.
Risk factors associated with ER+ breast cancer include low parity, late age of first
birth and early age of menarche. These reproductive traits all increase a woman’s
exposure to estrogen and support the modern mismatch hypothesis. In contrast, risk
factors for hormone negative breast cancer include early age of menarche, low SES
and decreased consumption of foods rich in micronutrients (i.e. less behavioral investment
in somatic maintenance).
Environmental cues during critical periods of development help to guide an organism’s
life history strategy. Environments with high extrinsic mortality and unpredictable
resources tend to select individuals with a fast life history strategy, that is, mature
and invest in reproduction earlier at the cost of growth and/or somatic maintenance
[4]. In contrast, secure, predictable environments delay reproduction and promote
investment in growth and maintenance, characteristics of a slow life history strategy.
Tumor suppression is a major component of somatic maintenance; this includes DNA repair,
cycle control and immune function [3]. Individuals that distribute resources to reproduction
at the cost of somatic maintenance may increase their risk for cancer through less
investment in DNA repair (leading to higher mutation rates) and/or immunosurveillance.
Accordingly, individuals with characteristics of a fast life history strategy may
have a fertility advantage early in life that is accompanied by increased cancer risk
later in life. There is preliminary evidence that suggests women at elevated risk
for hormone-independent breast cancer have a different reproductive profile. Women
who carry a germline mutation in BRCA1 or BRCA2 are more likely than non-carriers
to develop an ER− breast tumor [5]; carriers of these mutations were recently reported
to have significantly more children, shorter birth intervals and end their child-bearing
years later than aged-matched controls [6]. Although further research is needed to
confirm this observation, it is one of the first report to demonstrate a genetic link
between trade-offs in reproduction and breast cancer.
If women at risk for hormone-independent breast cancer invest more in reproduction,
we should expect to see evidence for less investment in somatic maintenance. An individual’s
overall cancer risk, including breast cancer, increases over their lifetime. One of
the most important epidemiologic patterns of breast tumor subtypes is that ER− tumors
are more common among younger women [1]. One potential explanation for the earlier
age of onset of ER− breast cancer could be faster biological aging due to less somatic
maintenance.
Environmental cues, such as resource predictability and extrinsic mortality, impact
an individual’s life strategy. SES provides an indicator of an individual’s environment
and resources. Interestingly, incidence of breast cancer subtypes varies along the
SES gradient. Compared to US women with other breast cancer subtypes, those diagnosed
with a hormone-independent tumor are more likely to report Black or Hispanic ethnicity
(versus non-Hispanic White) [1]. And, statistically independent of race and ethnicity,
ER− tumors are more common among women of lower SES [1]. These patterns suggest that
women from less privileged backgrounds have a higher relative risk of ER− breast cancer
compared with women that are (on average) reared in more stable and predictable environments.
As mentioned above, early environmental cues can steer an organism’s life history
strategy. In humans, age at first menstruation signals the switch from investment
in growth and/or maintenance to reproduction. In modern-industrialized samples, age
of menarche can be delayed by family warmth and paternal investment, while pubertal
timing can be accelerated in moderately stressful environments of nutritional adequacy
[7]. In USA, African American girls begin puberty earlier than White counterparts
[7]. An early age of menarche is associated with a higher risk of ER+ and ER− breast
cancer [2]. A low SES environment in childhood may be stressful enough to prompt an
accelerated life history strategy.
Eating nutritious food and leisure-time physical activity are examples of behavioral
somatic maintenance. Individuals of low SES report investing less in their health;
interestingly, this association was found to be completely mediated by perceived extrinsic
mortality risk [8]. Therefore, beyond economic barriers, high rates of violence in
low SES neighborhoods may contribute to decreased engagement health-promoting behavior.
A pooled analysis of nearly 1 million women followed for 11–20 years found that those
who ate more fruits and vegetables were less likely to develop ER− tumors compared
with those who ate fewer fruits and vegetables [9]. This effect was statistically
independent of ethnicity, education and 14 other potentially confounding risk factors
(e.g. alcohol consumption, body mass index and hormone replacement therapy) [9]. Surprisingly,
fruit and vegetable consumption was not found to be protective against ER+ breast
tumors. A similar pattern is observed for carbohydrate consumption; postmenopausal
women with a higher dietary glycemic load have an elevated risk of ER− breast cancer,
but not ER+ breast cancer [10]. Poor diet quality is more strongly associated with
ER− than with ER+ breast cancer.
Threatening stimuli early in life can increase the risk of adult-onset diseases via
epigenetic changes [11]. Although much of this literature is not framed evolutionarily,
these epigenetic profiles theoretically guide resources away from somatic maintenance,
that is, a faster life history strategy. Somatic maintenance is costly and genes involved
in tumor suppression are frequently downregulated via epigenetic silencing in breast
tumors [12]. Stress-induced epigenetic changes may in turn be exacerbated by a low
intake of fruits and vegetables, which are rich sources of micronutrients and other
bioactive compounds. Food components can influence epigenetic processes multitudinously;
for example, several vitamins participate in the methyl cycle, thereby affecting DNA
methylation, and several phytochemicals are known to directly modulate histone acetylation
processes [13, 14]. We suggest that epigenetic alterations may be a mechanism that
links diet, the SES gradient and ER− breast cancer risk [15].
The proposed connection between life history strategy and ER− breast cancer risk implies
a few immediate predictions. Early life stressors that accelerate pubertal timing
may be associated with increased promoter methylation of tumor suppressor genes, including
those expressed in human breast epithelium. Women with hormone-independent breast
cancer often have BRCA1 promoter hypermethylation [12]. Information on early environment
(SES), reproductive traits (pubertal timing, age of first birth and number of offspring)
and epigenetic profiles could provide insight into the relationship between life history
strategy and breast cancer susceptibility. We also predict that other activities related
to low behavioral somatic maintenance, for example, alcohol consumption, smoking and
body fatness, would be associated with elevated hormone-independent breast cancer
risk. Although it will be difficult to untangle the effects of alcohol and body fatness
on breast cancer risk because both directly increase estrogen levels [16]. It is also
unclear how life history trade-offs in the host affect trade-offs in the tumor. In
vitro studies could help determine how ER+ and ER− breast cancer cells differ in life
history characteristics, such as division rate and death rate. Another challenge in
this domain of research is the heterogeneous expression of estrogen-receptor both
within and between tumors categorized as ER+. How this widespread methodology affects
our observations cannot currently be measured; the question would best be interrogated
by comparing risk factor differences between breast tumor subtypes from various categorization
schemes. Lastly, if hormone-independent breast cancer is a result of less somatic
maintenance, we would predict these tumors to have greater genetic heterogeneity (via
a higher mutation rate) and the individual to have a decreased immune function, which
is consistent with ER− tumors’ more aggressive phenotype.
In summary, there appear to be different risk factors for different types of breast
cancer. Greater cumulative estrogen exposure from delayed childbirth and fewer total
offspring is associated with ER+ breast cancer, a cost of modern reproductive behavior.
However, hormone-independent breast tumors exhibit a different profile of environmental
risk factors. ER− breast cancer susceptibility shows no association with parity or
age at first birth. Instead, genetic variants associated with higher fertility, racial
and ethnic minority membership, low SES and eating fewer fruits and vegetables (behavioral
somatic maintenance) are associated with a higher risk for ER− breast cancer. An accelerated
life history strategy lends a framework for explaining these associations that will
hopefully lead to clinical application, but at this stage, more mechanistic evidence
is needed. Future research into the epigenetic effects of early psychosocial stress
and malnutrition could enhance understanding of why some women are more vulnerable
to the most lethal subtype of breast cancer.