In most languages, sentences can be broken down into words, which themselves can be
further decomposed into units that contain meaning of their own, so-called morphemes
(e.g., “play” or plural form “-s”). Morphemes are the main building blocks and tools,
which we use to create and change words. The representation of morphologically complex
words (inflected, derived, and compound) in the mental lexicon and their neurocognitive
processing has been a vigorously investigated topic in psycholinguistics and the cognitive
neuroscience of language. Are morphologically complex words such as “player” and “plays”
decomposed into their constituents (i.e., into their stem “play” and plural suffix
“-s” or agentive suffix “-er”) or are they processed and represented holistically
(“player” and “plays”)? Despite extensive research, many important questions remain
unanswered. Our Research Topic addresses several currently unresolved topics on the
time-course of morphological analysis and the relationship between form and meaning
information in morphological parsing. The studies also seek answers to the questions
of how inflections and derivations differ in the way they are handled by the mental
lexicon, how compound words are recognized and produced, as well as how morphologically
complex words are processed within the bilingual mental lexicon, as well as by different
clinical populations.
With respect to time-course of morphological processing and interplay between form
and meaning, many current models assume that morphological processing proceeds by
analyzing form first at the very earliest stages of processing, after which meaning
of the morphemes is accessed (e.g., Rastle and Davis, 2008). In contrast, Feldman
et al. provided evidence for the view that meaning information comes into play even
at the very early stages of morphologically complex word recognition. Two studies
(Estivalet and Meunier; Smolka et al.), focusing on the role of semantic transparency
and regularity in derived and inflected words indicate decomposition in semantically
and phonologically opaque and transparent words in two different languages. That is,
both semantically transparent and opaque derivations were found to be represented
and processed in similar ways in German (Smolka et al.), and all inflected verbal
forms in French showed decomposition effects during visual recognition (Estivalet
and Meunier), regardless of their regularity and phonological realization, thus supporting
models of obligatory morphological decomposition (e.g., Taft, 2004). Two neuroimaging
studies in this Research Topic elucidated the neural correlates of the processing
of regular vs. irregular inflection, a highly debated issue. Using time-resolved magnetoencephalography
(MEG) with English verbs, Fruchter et al. found priming effects for visually presented
irregular stimuli, quite early in the processing, within the left fusiform and inferior
temporal regions. The results were interpreted as favoring a single mechanism account
of the English past tense, in which even irregulars are decomposed into stems and
affixes prior to lexical access (Stockall and Marantz, 2006), as opposed to a dual
mechanism model, in which irregulars are recognized as whole forms (e.g., Pinker,
1991). On the other hand, with Russian, a language with very little scrutiny so far
and a relatively novel analysis of fMRI functional connectivity, Kireev et al. reported
that functional connectivity between the left inferior frontal gyrus (LIFG) and bilateral
superior temporal gyri (STG) was significantly greater for regular real verbs than
for irregular ones during production. The results shed new light on the functional
interplay within the language-processing network and stress the role of functional
temporo-frontal connectivity in complex morphological processes. These two studies
with arguably different outcomes suggest that the debate on regular vs. irregular
form processing continues. They however also point to the potentially critical influences
of the processing modality (written vs. spoken) as well as the task (comprehension
vs. production) on the mechanism of morphological processing.
Turning to a question of inflected and derived word processing, where several previous
studies have observed differences in the underpinning neural mechanisms (e.g., Leminen
et al.; Leminen et al., 2013; Leminen et al., for a review see e.g., Bozic and Marslen-Wilson,
2010). Service and Maury report differences between derivations and inflections in
working memory (as measured by simple and complex span tasks), suggesting different
levels of lexical competition and hence, differential lexical storage. Using combined
magneto- and electroencephalography (M/EEG), Whiting et al. defined the spatiotemporal
patterns of activity that support the recognition of spoken English inflectional and
derivational words. Results demonstrated that spoken complex word processing engages
the left-hemisphere's fronto-temporal language network, and, importantly, does not
require focused attention on the linguistic input (Whiting et al.). Using a similar
auditory passive oddball paradigm and EEG, Hanna and Pulvermuller observed that the
processing of spoken derived words was governed by a distributed set of bilateral
temporo—parietal areas, consistent with the previous literature (Bozic et al., 2013;
Leminen et al.). In addition, derived words were found to have full-form memory traces
in the neural lexicon (see e.g., Clahsen et al., 2003; Bozic and Marslen-Wilson, 2010;
Leminen et al.), activated automatically (see also Leminen et al., 2013).
In the field of cognitive neuroscience of language, a largely under-investigated topic
has been the neural processing of compound words. An article by Brooks and Cid da
Garcia therefore brings an important contribution to elucidating this issue. Their
primed word naming task revealed decompositional effects in access to both transparent
and opaque compounds. In the MEG results, the left anterior temporal lobe (LATL) as
well as the left posterior superior temporal gyrus showed increased activity only
for the transparent compounds. These effects were concluded to be related to compositional
processes and lexical-semantic retrieval, respectively. Our Research Topic also presents
novel findings on written production of compounds, where Bertram et al. introduces
an approach rarely used with morphologically complex words. Specifically, they investigated
the interplay between central linguistic processing and peripheral motor processes
during typewriting. Bertram et al. concluded that compound words seem to be retrieved
as whole words before writing is initiated and that linguistic planning is not fully
complete before writing, but cascades into the motor execution phase.
With respect to the important topic on bilingual morphological processing, our Research
Topic presents three studies and one commentary. Lensink et al. used a priming paradigm
to show that both transparent (e.g., moonlight) and opaque (e.g., honeymoon) compounds
in the second language (L2) undergo morphological analysis in production. The second
study (De Grauwe et al.) used fMRI to assess the processing of Dutch prefixed derived
words, demonstrating a priming effect for L2 speakers in the LIFG, an area that has
been associated with morphological decomposition. De Grauwe et al. concluded that
L2 speakers decompose transparent derived verbs rather than process them holistically.
In his commentary on De Grauwe et al.'s article, Jacob discusses the specific aspect
of decomposition that the LIFG finding might be reflecting, as well as the extent
to which the findings can be generalized to all derivations, instead of one particular
verb class. In the third article, Mulder et al. examined the role of orthography and
task-related processing mechanisms in the activation of morphologically related complex
words during bilingual word processing. Their study shows that the combined morphological
family size is a better predictor of reaction times (RTs) than the family size of
individual languages. This study also demonstrates that the effect of morphological
family size is sensitive to both semantic and orthographic factors, and that it also
depends on task demands.
Last but not least, two studies aimed to provide insights into morphological processing
by analyzing neglect and letter position issues in dyslexic population. Reznick and
Friedmann suggested that the effect of morphology on reading patterns in neglexia
provides supportive evidence that morphological decomposition occurs pre-lexically,
in an early orthographic-visual analysis stage. Using a different dyslexic population,
letter position dyslexics, Friedmann et al. reached a similar conclusion that morphological
parsing takes place at an early, pre-lexical stage and that decomposition is structurally
rather than lexically driven.
To summarize, this Research Topic presents an overview of a wide range of questions
currently addressed in the field of morphological processing. It highlights the significance
of morphological information in language processing, both written and spoken, as assessed
by the variety of methods and approaches presented here. The partly discrepant findings
in some of the contributions to our Research Topic also underline the need for increased
cross-talk between researchers using different methods, modalities, and paradigms.
Author contributions
AL wrote the main paper, ML and MB edited the manuscript, HC provided conceptual advice.
Conflict of interest statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial
or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.