7
views
0
recommends
+1 Recommend
0 collections
    0
    shares
      • Record: found
      • Abstract: found
      • Article: found
      Is Open Access

      Feeling cold is contagious

      article-commentary
      Temperature: Multidisciplinary Biomedical Journal
      Taylor & Francis

      Read this article at

      Bookmark
          There is no author summary for this article yet. Authors can add summaries to their articles on ScienceOpen to make them more accessible to a non-specialist audience.

          Abstract

          Comment on: Cooper EA, et al. You turn me cold: Evidence for temperature contagion. PLoS One 2014; 9:e116126; http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0116126 Maintaining a stable internal thermal environment is critical to many life-preserving biological processes. Consequently, core body temperature is rigidly regulated by the brains of all homeothermic animals through a variety of involuntary thermoregulatory responses including cutaneous vasomotor responses, shivering, sweating and piloerection as well as behavioral responses such as moving into the sun or shade. Though bottom-up pathways play a central thermoregulatory role, studies using visual imagery, temperature biofeedback and hypnotic suggestion illustrate top-down influences. We have recently extended these findings by showing that thermoregulatory responses can also be modulated by simple observation of another's temperature change. 1 A finding similar to effects studied under the rubric of facial feedback. To investigate whether observing another's temperature change alters observers own peripheral body temperature we showed thirty-six healthy participants 8 purpose-made 3-minute videos depicting actors with their right or left hand in visibly warm (warm video) or cold (cold video) water. During observation of the videos participants' own right and left hand temperature was measured using custom thermometers with a theoretical temperature sensitivity of 0.0001°C. After video playback participants rated how warm or cold the actors hand appeared. Four control videos with the actors' hand placed in front of the water container were also shown. Participants rated videos showing hands immersed in cold water as being significantly cooler than hands immersed in warm water confirming that they could perceive the differences in observed hand temperature (Fig. 1A). However, more interestingly we also observed a significant change in participants' own hand temperature during videos depicting another's hand immersed in cold/warm water but not control videos. Specifically, participants' own hands became significantly colder when observing cold compared to warm videos F(1,34) = 13.83, p = 0.001. Further exploration showed that this was predominantly driven by reductions in participants' own left (t(35) = 23.54, p = 0.001) and right (t(35) = 22.33, p = 0.026) hand temperature during cold videos (Fig. 1C). Though warm hand videos were associated with an early increase in observers own hand temperature this was not sustained and did not significantly change over total video playback (Fig. 1D). Figure 1. (A) Participants mean subjective ratings for the observed temperature of the actor's hand (“How Hot or Cold is the Actor's Hand?”) reported using a keyboard controlled visual analog scale ranging from ‘Very Cold’ (far left) through ‘Neutral’ (center) to ‘Very Hot’ (far right). (B) Schematic of how cortical (top down) influences may influence thermoregulation. (C) Mean time course response to viewing all cold videos for the left (dark blue) and right (light blue) hand displayed in 10 s epochs. Data are overlaid on a representative frame from one of the cold videos. (D) Mean time course response to viewing all warm videos for the left (red) and right (orange) hand displayed in 10 s epochs. Data are overlaid on a representative frame from one of the warm videos. Data shown are from ref. 1. The neural circuitry underlying this ‘contagion’ of others peripheral body temperature is currently unknown. Prior brain imaging studies have shown that direct hand cooling increases blood flow (an indirect measure of neuronal activity) within human posterior then subsequently middle and anterior insula. 2 This finding is noteworthy, as insula is believed to provide cortical representations of bodily physiological state across physiological domains. 3 Whether similar increases in insula activity are also associated with temperature changes observed in others is unknown, though studies showing shared neuronal circuitry (including insula) in the direct and vicarious experience of a related physiological response, pain, suggest they would. 4 In this regard, it is instructive to note that in monkeys many thermosensitive neurons in the hypothalamic preoptic area are also sensitive to non-thermal emotional stimuli such as rewards or aversive stimuli. 5 This suggests that effector pathways from the preoptic area to rostral medullary raphe may be recruited by top-down cognitive processes potentially including both anterior insula and anterior cingulate cortices (Fig. 1B). Interestingly, different thermoregulatory effectors mechanisms are associated with partially separable central control systems, expressed physiologically as a greater sensitivity of vasoconstrictive responses to temperature change. 6 This difference in central control mechanism may also underpin why, in our study, we saw isolated changes in hand temperature, which we predict are likely mediated by a direct preoptic to rostral medullary raphe pathway, but not heart rate which is mediated by an intermediate projection to the dorsomedial hypothalamus. 6 WARM sensitive neurons projecting via the dorsal parabrachial nucleus play a similar role in orchestrating cutaneous vasodilation and tachycardia in response to environmental warming. Insight into the mechanisms underlying temperature contagion may also be usefully informed by studies of disrupted body ownership induced experimentally using the rubber hand illusion (where simultaneous stroking of a visible rubber hand and hidden own hand create the illusion of rubber hand ownership), and the clinical disorder cold-type complex regional pain syndrome, a neurological disorder associated with pain, abnormal temperature regulation and often dystonia in a single limb. In both conditions unilateral disruption of body ownership is associated with a localized reduction in body temperature suggesting that the conscious sense of our physical self and its physiological regulation are linked. 7 During experimental induction of the rubber hand illusion activity changes are observed within insula as well as premotor and intra-parietal cortex suggesting a potential role for the insula in reported temperature changes. In cold-type complex regional pain syndrome, changes in limb temperature are reported to be dependent on its physical location in space. For example, when the affected (cool) limb is moved across the midline its temperature spontaneously increases with a converse effect described for the healthy limb. 7 Based on this finding, these authors argued for a space-based rather than self-related (somatotopic) frame of reference, with descending projections from parietal cortex onto brainstem autonomic centers hypothesized as the mechanism mediating spatially dependent temperature change. It is possible that inter-personal comparator processes within the intra-parietal junction play a similar role in the social contagion of temperature responses. A final finding from our study was a nuanced relationship between sensitivity to temperature contagion and self-reported empathy. Individuals high in empathic concern yet low in a broader measure of empathy (balanced emotional empathy scale) showed the greatest sensitivity to temperature contagion, broadly supporting action-perception models of empathy that propose embodiment of another's physiological state as a substrate for empathy. Why we observed this complex relationship rather than a simpler positive correlation with both types of empathy is currently unclear, though may relate to differences in the concepts captured by each scale. In conclusion, we show that healthy individuals are sensitive to observable signals of another's peripheral body temperature and further, show contagion of their temperature, particularly in the context of cold. Inter-individual differences in temperature contagion are marked and correlate with inter-individual difference in empathy. The neurobiological substrates underpinning these effects are currently unknown though prior studies suggest potential mediation via top-down influences on thermosensitive neurons within the preoptic area of the hypothalamus.

          Related collections

          Most cited references3

          • Record: found
          • Abstract: found
          • Article: found

          Spatially defined modulation of skin temperature and hand ownership of both hands in patients with unilateral complex regional pain syndrome.

          Numerous clinical conditions, including complex regional pain syndrome, are characterized by autonomic dysfunctions (e.g. altered thermoregulation, sometimes confined to a single limb), and disrupted cortical representation of the body and the surrounding space. The presence, in patients with complex regional pain syndrome, of a disruption in spatial perception, bodily ownership and thermoregulation led us to hypothesize that impaired spatial perception might result in a spatial-dependent modulation of thermoregulation and bodily ownership over the affected limb. In five experiments involving a total of 23 patients with complex regional pain syndrome of one arm and 10 healthy control subjects, we measured skin temperature of the hand with infrared thermal imaging, before and after experimental periods of either 9 or 10 min each, during which the hand was held on one or the other side of the body midline. Tactile processing was assessed by temporal order judgements of pairs of vibrotactile stimuli, delivered one to each hand. Pain and sense of ownership over the hand were assessed by self-report scales. Across experiments, when kept on its usual side of the body midline, the affected hand was 0.5 ± 0.3°C cooler than the healthy hand (P < 0.02 for all, a common finding in cold-type complex regional pain syndrome), and tactile stimuli delivered to the healthy hand were prioritized over those delivered to the affected hand. Simply crossing both hands over the midline resulted in (i) warming of the affected hand (the affected hand became 0.4 ± 0.3°C warmer than when it was in the uncrossed position; P = 0.01); (ii) cooling of the healthy hand (by 0.3 ± 0.3°C; P = 0.02); and (iii) reversal of the prioritization of tactile processing. When only the affected hand was crossed over the midline, it became warmer (by 0.5 ± 0.3°C; P = 0.01). When only the healthy hand was crossed over the midline, it became cooler (by 0.3 ± 0.3°C; P = 0.01). The temperature change of either hand was positively related to its distance from the body midline (pooled data: r = 0.76, P < 0.001). Crossing the affected hand over the body midline had small but significant effects on both spontaneous pain (which was reduced) and the sense of ownership over the hand (which was increased) (P < 0.04 for both). We conclude that impaired spatial perception modulated temperature of the limbs, tactile processing, spontaneous pain and the sense of ownership over the hands. These results show that complex regional pain syndrome involves more complex neurological dysfunction than has previously been considered.
            Bookmark
            • Record: found
            • Abstract: found
            • Article: found
            Is Open Access

            You Turn Me Cold: Evidence for Temperature Contagion

            Introduction During social interactions, our own physiological responses influence those of others. Synchronization of physiological (and behavioural) responses can facilitate emotional understanding and group coherence through inter-subjectivity. Here we investigate if observing cues indicating a change in another's body temperature results in a corresponding temperature change in the observer. Methods Thirty-six healthy participants (age; 22.9±3.1 yrs) each observed, then rated, eight purpose-made videos (3 min duration) that depicted actors with either their right or left hand in visibly warm (warm videos) or cold water (cold videos). Four control videos with the actors' hand in front of the water were also shown. Temperature of participant observers' right and left hands was concurrently measured using a thermistor within a Wheatstone bridge with a theoretical temperature sensitivity of 0.1). There was however no evidence of left-right mirroring of these temperature effects p>0.1). Sensitivity to temperature contagion was also predicted by inter-individual differences in self-report empathy. Conclusions We illustrate physiological contagion of temperature in healthy individuals, suggesting that empathetic understanding for primary low-level physiological challenges (as well as more complex emotions) are grounded in somatic simulation.
              Bookmark
              • Record: found
              • Abstract: found
              • Article: not found

              Responsiveness of monkey preoptic thermosensitive neurons to non-thermal emotional stimuli.

              Responsiveness of 143 preoptic neurons to changes in hypothalamic temperature and to non-thermal emotional stimuli were investigated while rewarding (foods) and aversive objects (hypertonic saline, a toy snake, an air puffer) were given. About 71% of thermosensitive neurons and 32% of thermally insensitive neurons changed the activity when emotional stimuli were shown to and/or tasted by the monkey. Such responses were modulated by satiety/hunger state and were dependent on the degree of perturbation of emotional state. About half of the neurons tested responded when the monkey opened the mouth and protruded the tongue or moved fingers in trying to obtain foods with strong motivation, but did not when the animal made such movements less readily or reluctantly with the progress of satiation. This response was most frequently found among warm-units. The results raise a possibility that preoptic thermosensitive neurons, besides their postulated thermoregulatory functions, might be involved in the response of coordination with thermal and non-thermal emotional behaviors controlled in the hypothalamus.
                Bookmark

                Author and article information

                Journal
                Temperature (Austin)
                Temperature (Austin)
                KTMP
                Temperature: Multidisciplinary Biomedical Journal
                Taylor & Francis
                2332-8940
                2332-8959
                Jan-Mar 2016
                5 May 2015
                5 May 2015
                : 3
                : 1
                : 20-22
                Affiliations
                Brighton and Sussex Medical School, University of Sussex , Brighton, UK
                Sackler Centre for Consciousness Science, University of Sussex , Brighton, UK
                Sussex Partnership NHS Trust , Brighton, UK
                Author notes
                Article
                1042567
                10.1080/23328940.2015.1042567
                4861179
                27227076
                849acccf-ba65-4b7d-a639-4636355724a6
                © 2016 The Author(s). Published with license by Taylor & Francis Group, LLC

                This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-Non-Commercial License http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/3.0/, which permits unrestricted non-commercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. The moral rights of the named author(s) have been asserted.

                History
                Page count
                Figures: 1, Tables: 0, References: 7, Pages: 3
                Categories
                Front Matter: Discoveries

                Comments

                Comment on this article