58
views
0
recommends
+1 Recommend
0 collections
    0
    shares
      • Record: found
      • Abstract: found
      • Article: found
      Is Open Access

      ENLIGHT: A consensus checklist for reporting laboratory-based studies on the non-visual effects of light in humans

      research-article

      Read this article at

      ScienceOpenPublisherPMC
          There is no author summary for this article yet. Authors can add summaries to their articles on ScienceOpen to make them more accessible to a non-specialist audience.

          Summary

          Background

          There is no consensus on reporting light characteristics in studies investigating non-visual responses to light. This project aimed to develop a reporting checklist for laboratory-based investigations on the impact of light on non-visual physiology.

          Methods

          A four-step modified Delphi process (three questionnaire-based feedback rounds and one face-to-face group discussion) involving international experts was conducted to reach consensus on the items to be included in the checklist. Following the consensus process, the resulting checklist was tested in a pilot phase with independent experts.

          Findings

          An initial list of 61 items related to reporting light-based interventions was condensed to a final checklist containing 25 items, based upon consensus among experts (final n = 60). Nine items were deemed necessary to report regardless of research question or context. A description of each item is provided in the accompanying Explanation and Elaboration (E&E) document. The independent pilot testing phase led to minor textual clarifications in the checklist and E&E document.

          Interpretation

          The ENLIGHT Checklist is the first consensus-based checklist for documenting and reporting ocular light-based interventions for human studies. The implementation of the checklist will enhance the impact of light-based research by ensuring comprehensive documentation, enhancing reproducibility, and enabling data aggregation across studies.

          Funding

          Network of European Institutes for Advanced Study (NETIAS) Constructive Advanced Thinking (CAT) programme; Sir Henry doi 10.13039/100004440, Wellcome; Postdoctoral Fellowship (Wellcome Trust, 204686/Z/16/Z); doi 10.13039/501100001826, Netherlands Organisation for Health Research and Development; VENI fellowship (2020–09150161910128); doi 10.13039/100000005, U.S. Department of Defense; Grant (W81XWH-16-1-0223); doi 10.13039/501100001352, National University of Singapore; (NUHSRO/2022/038/Startup/08); and doi 10.13039/501100001381, National Research Foundation Singapore; (NRF2022-THE004-0002).

          Related collections

          Most cited references35

          • Record: found
          • Abstract: found
          • Article: not found

          Defining consensus: a systematic review recommends methodologic criteria for reporting of Delphi studies.

          To investigate how consensus is operationalized in Delphi studies and to explore the role of consensus in determining the results of these studies. Systematic review of a random sample of 100 English language Delphi studies, from two large multidisciplinary databases [ISI Web of Science (Thompson Reuters, New York, NY) and Scopus (Elsevier, Amsterdam, NL)], published between 2000 and 2009. About 98 of the Delphi studies purported to assess consensus, although a definition for consensus was only provided in 72 of the studies (64 a priori). The most common definition for consensus was percent agreement (25 studies), with 75% being the median threshold to define consensus. Although the authors concluded in 86 of the studies that consensus was achieved, consensus was only specified a priori (with a threshold value) in 42 of these studies. Achievement of consensus was related to the decision to stop the Delphi study in only 23 studies, with 70 studies terminating after a specified number of rounds. Although consensus generally is felt to be of primary importance to the Delphi process, definitions of consensus vary widely and are poorly reported. Improved criteria for reporting of methods of Delphi studies are required. Copyright © 2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
            • Record: found
            • Abstract: not found
            • Article: not found

            The Delphi method as a research tool: an example, design considerations and applications

              • Record: found
              • Abstract: found
              • Article: not found

              Melanopsin-containing retinal ganglion cells: architecture, projections, and intrinsic photosensitivity.

              The primary circadian pacemaker, in the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) of the mammalian brain, is photoentrained by light signals from the eyes through the retinohypothalamic tract. Retinal rod and cone cells are not required for photoentrainment. Recent evidence suggests that the entraining photoreceptors are retinal ganglion cells (RGCs) that project to the SCN. The visual pigment for this photoreceptor may be melanopsin, an opsin-like protein whose coding messenger RNA is found in a subset of mammalian RGCs. By cloning rat melanopsin and generating specific antibodies, we show that melanopsin is present in cell bodies, dendrites, and proximal axonal segments of a subset of rat RGCs. In mice heterozygous for tau-lacZ targeted to the melanopsin gene locus, beta-galactosidase-positive RGC axons projected to the SCN and other brain nuclei involved in circadian photoentrainment or the pupillary light reflex. Rat RGCs that exhibited intrinsic photosensitivity invariably expressed melanopsin. Hence, melanopsin is most likely the visual pigment of phototransducing RGCs that set the circadian clock and initiate other non-image-forming visual functions.

                Author and article information

                Contributors
                Journal
                eBioMedicine
                EBioMedicine
                eBioMedicine
                Elsevier
                2352-3964
                02 December 2023
                December 2023
                02 December 2023
                : 98
                : 104889
                Affiliations
                [a ]TUM School of Medicine & Health, Department of Health and Sport Sciences, Technical University of Munich, Munich, Germany
                [b ]TUM Institute for Advanced Study (TUM-IAS), Technical University of Munich, Garching, Germany
                [c ]Max Planck Institute for Biological Cybernetics, Max Planck Research Group Translational Sensory & Circadian Neuroscience, Tübingen, Germany
                [d ]TUMCREATE, Singapore, Singapore
                [e ]Laboratory for Neurophysiology, Department of Cellular and Chemical Biology, Leiden University Medical Center, Leiden, Netherlands
                [f ]Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University, Stanford, USA
                [g ]Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, Victoria, Australia
                [h ]School of Psychological Science and Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Monash University, Melbourne, Australia
                [i ]Department of Ophthalmology and Department of Biomedical Engineering, National University of Singapore, Singapore, Singapore
                [j ]Center for Innovation & Precision Eye Health, National University of Singapore, Singapore, Singapore
                [k ]Singapore Eye Research Institute, Singapore, Singapore
                [l ]Ophthalmology and Visual Sciences Academic Clinical Programme, Duke-NUS Medical School, Singapore, Singapore
                Author notes
                []Corresponding author. TUM School of Medicine & Health, Department of Health and Sport Sciences, Technical University of Munich, Munich, Germany. manuel.spitschan@ 123456tum.de
                [∗∗ ]Corresponding author. Laboratory for Neurophysiology, Department of Cellular and Chemical Biology, Leiden University Medical Center, Leiden, Netherlands. L.Kervezee@ 123456lumc.nl
                [∗∗∗ ]Corresponding author. Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University, Stanford, USA. rlok@ 123456stanford.edu
                [∗∗∗∗ ]Corresponding author. Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, Victoria, Australia. elise.mcglashan@ 123456unimelb.edu.au
                [∗∗∗∗∗ ]Corresponding author. Department of Ophthalmology and Department of Biomedical Engineering, National University of Singapore, Singapore, Singapore. rpnajjar@ 123456nus.edu.sg
                [m]

                Equal contribution.

                [n]

                The full list of ENLIGHT Consortium members can be found in Supplemental Table S2 of this manuscript.

                Article
                S2352-3964(23)00455-3 104889
                10.1016/j.ebiom.2023.104889
                10704221
                38043137
                918ec13b-a770-432e-8460-0e168ac9256b
                © 2023 The Author(s)

                This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).

                History
                : 16 April 2023
                : 19 October 2023
                : 10 November 2023
                Categories
                Articles

                reporting guidelines,non-visual effects of light,circadian rhythms,sleep,interventions,light

                Comments

                Comment on this article

                Related Documents Log