10
views
0
recommends
+1 Recommend
0 collections
    0
    shares
      • Record: found
      • Abstract: found
      • Article: found
      Is Open Access

      Perspectives on Communicating Biomarker-Based Assessments of Alzheimer’s Disease to Cognitively Healthy Individuals

      review-article

      Read this article at

      Bookmark
          There is no author summary for this article yet. Authors can add summaries to their articles on ScienceOpen to make them more accessible to a non-specialist audience.

          Abstract

          In clinical trials which target pathophysiological mechanisms associated with Alzheimer’s disease, research participants who are recruited based on biomarker test results should be informed about their increased risk of developing Alzheimer’s dementia. This paper presents the results of a qualitative focus group study of attitudes and concerns toward learning information about biomarker-based risk status among healthy research participants in the United Kingdom and Spain and people with dementia and their supporters/caregivers from countries represented in the European Working Group of People with Dementia of Alzheimer Europe. The study identified expectations related to learning risk status and preferences related to the content, quality, and follow-up of the disclosure process. The latter emphasize distinctions between risk and diagnoses, the importance of clear information about risk, and suggestions for risk reduction, as well as expectations for follow up and support. The implications of these preferences for practice are discussed. Providing details of research participants’ experience and views may serve as a guide for the development of processes for the responsible disclosure of Alzheimer’s disease biomarkers.

          Related collections

          Most cited references42

          • Record: found
          • Abstract: found
          • Article: found
          Is Open Access

          Amyloid imaging in cognitively normal individuals, at-risk populations and preclinical Alzheimer's disease☆

          1 Introduction This review will focus on amyloid imaging in cognitively normal elderly, asymptomatic at-risk populations, and individuals with subjective cognitive decline. It is one of two side-to-side review papers, the second one by Vandenberghe (2013--this issue) focusing on amyloid imaging in cognitively impaired populations. The present effort extends from a talk presented at the Alzheimer's Association International Conference (http://www.alz.org/aaic/overview.asp) in July 2012 on amyloid imaging in preclinical individuals. It will cover the prevalence of amyloid-positive cases amongst cognitively normal elderly, the influence of risk factors for AD, the relationships to cognition, atrophy and prognosis, longitudinal amyloid imaging and ethical aspects related to amyloid imaging in cognitively normal individuals. The goal was not to be exhaustive but to give weighted opinions on most challenging contemporary debates based on our current state of knowledge. Thus, some topics will not be covered, such as the relationships with other brain imaging modalities (e.g. fluoro-deoxyglucose (FDG)-PET, task-related and resting-state functional MRI, diffusion tensor imaging) and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) biomarkers, or discussion on the similarities and differences between the various PET amyloid ligands. β-amyloid (Aβ) deposition is one of the main hallmarks of Alzheimer's disease and is thought to play a central role in the neurodegenerative process characterizing this disease (Hardy and Selkoe, 2002; Masters et al., 2006). Neuropathological studies have shown more than 20 years ago that substantial level of Aβ deposition can be found in the autopsied brain of cases with documented normal cognition (Braak and Braak, 1997; Crystal et al., 1988; Katzman et al., 1988; Price and Morris, 1999). Recently, PET amyloid ligands have been developed, the first one (except from FDDNP see below) being the 11C-Pittsburgh Compound B (11C-PIB) PET ligand (Klunk et al., 2004), followed by the recently Food and Drug Administration (FDA)-approved 18F-florbetapir (Choi et al., 2009; Wong et al., 2010) and other 18F-labeled ligands (Herholz and Ebmeier, 2011). Thanks to these developments, we entered a new exciting area where it is possible to visualize plaques in the brain of living participants. This offers the unique opportunity to get further – including longitudinal – information in these individuals, so as to improve our understanding of the consequence of the presence of Aβ deposition in the brain of cognitively normal elderly, and more generally of the role of Aβ deposition in early AD pathological processes. Note that studies will be reviewed in what follows irrespective of the PET amyloid ligand being used, with the exception of studies using FDDNP (e.g. Small et al., 2006) that will not be included here as we aimed at specifically addressing issues related to Aβ while FDDNP binds to both Aβ and tau abnormalities. 2 The presence of Aβ in the brain of cognitively normal elderly and at-risk populations 2.1 The prevalence of amyloid-positive cases within cognitively normal elderly Consistent with neuropathological studies (Price and Morris, 1999), neuroimaging amyloid-PET studies found amyloid-positive cases within cognitively normal (“healthy”) older people. The first in-vivo 11C-PIB PET study reported one 11C-PIB-positive case amongst the control elderly (Klunk et al., 2004), and this has been consistently reported since then. A bimodal distribution of neocortical 11C-PIB values is usually reported within elderly subjects with normal cognition (e.g. Klunk, 2011), though there is recent and accumulating evidence for intermediate cases (see below). A majority of healthy elderly shows low 11C-PIB retention, but part of them shows distinctly elevated 11C-PIB retention in regions that ultimately develop heavy Aβ loads in AD patients, especially the posterior cingulate cortex – precuneus and the anterior cingulate cortex – medial orbitofronal cortex (Aizenstein et al., 2008; Archer et al., 2006; Bourgeat et al., 2010; Dickerson et al., 2009; Hatashita and Yamasaki, 2010; Jack et al., 2008; Mintun et al., 2006; Morris et al., 2010; Nelissen et al., 2007; Price and Morris, 1999; Rowe et al., 2007; Storandt et al., 2009). While neuropathological and amyloid-PET neuroimaging studies have thus consistently demonstrated that some elderly with normal cognition may have Aβ deposition in their brain, what is less consensual is the prevalence of cognitively normal elderly with an amyloid-positive scan. Extremely variable proportions have been reported in the literacy ranging from 0% (Okello et al., 2009) to 47% (Jagust et al., 2010), with prevalence of 10 to 30% being more frequently reported (Quigley et al., 2011) (see Table 1 for examples). Several factors are likely to explain this considerable variability. This could reflect methodological differences across studies (e.g. the amyloid ligand or the method used to define a positivity threshold), or genuine differences due to the samples and reflecting differences in the screening process or in genetic, social, ethnical and environmental factors (see “The influence of at-risk factors” below). Actually, the particular PET amyloid ligand that is used is probably not the main element to account for this large variability, as several studies reported a very good correlation between the different PET amyloid ligands (Johnson et al., 2013; Vandenberghe et al., 2010; Villemagne et al., 2012) (see also the side article by Vandenberghe et al. 2013--in this issue, for further details). By contrast, the method used to define positivity probably accounts for a significant part of this variability. They are clearly negative and clearly positive cases but there are also intermediate cases (Fig. 1). As further discussed below, these intermediate cases represent a non-negligible proportion of the cognitively normal elderly and their classification as positive or negative is highly sensitive to the method, which will thus significantly impact on the proportion of amyloid-positive scans. Note that not much is known about these intermediate cases, and this would be an important topic for future research. One previous work showed that intermediate cortical PIB values seem to reflect both lower number of elevated PIB regions and lower PIB value in these regions, through in the same network, as compared to the clearly positive cases (Mormino et al., 2012). However, further studies are needed, notably longitudinal studies to follow the progression of amyloid deposition in these intermediate individuals as well as to assess their risk of conversion as compared to the positive and negative categories. There are many methodological factors that may influence the classification of cases (see Edison et al., 2011 for example): the method used to read the scan (either through visual inspection or using quantitative values), the regions that are considered, the values that are used, e.g. corrected from partial volume effects or not, scaled using the pons or the cerebellum or another region, etc. It has been proposed for example that the pons may be more suitable as a reference region in specific cases, e.g. for longitudinal studies (Villain et al., 2012) or when amyloid deposition may be present in the cerebellum (e.g., in early-onset familial AD) (Edison et al., 2012; Fleisher et al., 2012). Another determining factor is the method used to define the threshold from which a scan is classified as positive or negative. Numerous methods have been used in the literature: clustering analyses, the 95th percentile, the iterative outlier approach, an absolute cut-off (e.g. SUVR > 1.50), the mean + 2 standard deviation (SD) of healthy elderly controls, and the mean + 2SD of healthy young controls (supposedly devoid of Aβ deposition), for a non-exhaustive list. The study by Mormino et al. (2012) is a good illustration of this point, as it showed that when using two different methods to define the threshold (i.e. the iterative outlier approach versus the mean + 2SD of young healthy controls), the percentage of 11C-PIB-positive individuals amongst healthy elderly varied considerably (from 15 to 35%) (see also Aizenstein et al., 2008). In the IMAP project conducted in the Inserm U1077 Unit in Caen (France), 3 out of 36 (8%) cognitively normal elderly were clearly positive (i.e. showed Aβ load in the range of AD patients). Only these 3 cases were classified as amyloid-positive using the iterative outlier approach (Fig. 1), while 9 additional cases were classified positively when using a group of 12 participants younger than 55 yrs (under the assumption that these individuals have no Aβ deposition and therefore the corresponding PET signal should only reflect noise). As a whole, intermediate cases can be as frequent as 20–25% in cognitively normal elderly populations and they may be responsible for a large part of the variability in the percentage of amyloid-positive cases. This is however not the only reason for differences in the proportion of amyloid-positive elderly: the screening procedure and selection criteria used in the different studies probably also account for a large part of this variability. Several factors are known to influence the proportion of amyloid-positive cases as discussed in the following section, and these factors may be more or less represented or controlled for according to the studies. 2.2 The effects of age and ApoE4 The two main risk factors for AD, namely age and ApoE4, have been consistently shown to have a significant impact on Aβ deposition in normal elderly (Mielke et al., 2012). For example, the prevalence of amyloid-positive cases within healthy older participants raised from 18% in the seventh decade to 60% in those over 80 yrs (Rowe et al., 2010) or from 0% at ages 45–49 yrs to 30% in the eighth decade in another study (Morris et al., 2010). Note that a linear relationship was found between Aβ deposition and age even within the 11C-PIB-negative cases when assessing a wide age range (23–80 years) (Vandenberghe et al., 2010). Similarly, amongst cognitively normal elderly, 49% of ApoE4 carriers were 11C-PIB-positive while they were only 21% within the non-carriers (Rowe et al., 2010). This effect is reported in many studies and is found to be dose-dependent and region-specific, i.e. to be more pronounced in some brain regions (such as temporo-parietal areas) than in others (Morris et al., 2010; Fleisher et al., 2013; Kantarci et al., 2012; Reiman et al., 2009; Villemagne et al., 2011). Age and ApoE are likely to account for part of the variability in the proportion of amyloid-positive cases as there are great differences between some studies/samples (e.g. 43% ApoE4 in Rowe et al., 2010 versus 22% in Doraiswamy et al., 2012, and a mean age of 69.8 years old in Rowe et al., 2010 versus 78 years old in Jagust et al., 2010). 2.3 Individuals with subjective cognitive decline Individuals with subjective cognitive decline are elderly who present with a cognitive complaint but do not show any significant cognitive deficit compared to subjects their age. This is a rather broad definition that may refer to many different entities as consensual criteria for subjective cognitive decline are missing to date. The presence or not of individuals with subjective cognitive decline is another factor that may influence the proportion of amyloid-positive cases in elderly cohorts as this criteria is not always controlled for. Thus, Perrotin et al. (2012) showed increased proportion of 11C-PIB positive cases amongst elderly who consider that their memory is the same or worse relative to people their age, compared to those who think their memory is better. A relationship between a subjective memory complaints composite score and cortical PiB binding has also been reported (Amariglio et al., 2012), but other reports found no significant difference in global neocortical 11C-PIB between healthy elderly with and without subjective cognitive decline (Chételat et al., 2010a). The significance of the effect thus likely depends on the cohort and the method to determine amyloid-positivity (see above) as well as to assess subjective cognitive decline. The different risk-factors may also interact, as suggested for example by the finding that subjective cognitive decline was only associated with elevated 11C-PIB binding in ApoE4 carriers (Rowe et al., 2010). 2.4 The effects of other genetic and environmental factors A familial, and especially maternal, history of AD has also been reported to be associated with increased 11C-PIB SUVR (Mosconi et al., 2010). This effect was shown to be independent from that of ApoE4 (Xiong et al., 2011), suggesting that non-APOE susceptibility genes for AD influence AD biomarkers. In the same line, a very interesting study by Scheinin et al. (2011) assessing cognitively preserved monozygotic and dizygotic cotwins of persons with AD showed that cognitively normal dizygotic cotwins had normal low 11C-PIB SUVR, while the monozygotic cognitively normal cotwins had abnormally elevated SUVR, almost at the level of their AD cotwins. This suggests that genetic factors at least partly determine the development of Aβ plaques, but also that there may be environmental/acquired factors that modulate the relationship between brain amyloidosis and cognition. This view agrees with studies highlighting the effect of education (Rentz et al., 2010), lifetime cognitive engagement (Landau et al., 2012), and physical exercise (Head et al., 2012; Liang et al., 2010) on 11C-PIB deposition or on its association to cognition or neuronal injury. In the same line, ApoE4 carriers who engaged in moderate levels of exercise had a lower amyloid burden than ApoE4 carriers with lower levels of exercise and this effect of exercise was not seen in the noncarriers (Head et al., 2012). While the effects of these different factors are not clear-cut, with some discrepancies between studies, they overall indicate that, consistent with the reserve theory (Stern, 2002), higher reserve proxies are associated with reduced amyloidosis or Aβ-related cognitive or neuronal deficits. 2.5 Asymptomatic mutation carriers for the early-onset familial form of AD Finally, further insights in this question arise from studies on the early onset familial form of AD (EOFAD). Thus, studies conducted in carriers of mutations that lead to EOFAD showed that increased amyloid load can be detected at a presymptomatic stage (Klunk et al., 2007; Knight et al., 2011; Villemagne et al., 2009). Interestingly, the topographical pattern is slightly different from that observed in sporadic AD (Fig. 2), with a predominance of Aβ deposition in the striatum of asymptomatic EOFAD while the neocortex is less systematically and less significantly involved than in sporadic AD, independently of mutation type (Klunk et al., 2007; Knight et al., 2011; Villemagne et al., 2009) (see Rinne and Någren, 2010; Berti et al., 2011 for reviews). Increased 11C-PIB binding has also been reported in the thalamus and the cerebellum in asymptomatic EOFAD (Knight et al., 2011; Villemagne et al., 2009). As for the timing and sequence of the apparition of brain Aβ deposition, a recent publication in EOFAD showed that Aβ deposition can be detected 15 years before expected symptom onset – corresponding to the parental age at onset as determined by a semistructured interview in which family members were asked about the age of first progressive cognitive decline (Bateman et al., 2012). This was also true for increased CSF tau and brain atrophy, while changes in CSF Aβ-42 were detected 25 years before, and hypometabolism and memory deficits 10 years before expected symptom onset. This is a very informative study from the Dominantly Inherited Alzheimer Network (DIAN) collaborative study gathering the largest MRI and PET multicentre database on this population. These findings were confirmed and extended in two other studies from a large Columbian kindred suggesting that neurodegenerative changes could precede or at least accompany evidence of Aβ deposition (Fleisher et al., 2012; Reiman et al., 2012). These results are crucial as they question the prevailing amyloid hypothesis and current models of the dynamic and sequence of the different biomarkers (Craig-Schapiro et al., 2009; Frisoni et al., 2010; Jack et al., 2010; Petersen, 2010; Weiner et al., 2010) that predict that Aβ deposition occurs first and is responsible for neurodegeneration. However, generalization to the common sporadic form of AD from results obtained in familial AD should be considered with caution. Results from comparable studies in preclinical sporadic AD (such as the ADNI or AIBL cohorts) and others, are still warranted to determine the sequence and timing of biomarkers in sporadic AD (see also below). Altogether, many genetic risk factors involved in familial or sporadic AD were found to influence Aβ deposition, suggesting that Aβ load is highly heritable (Berti et al., 2011). However, healthy life and stimulating environment seem to allow delaying/reducing Aβ deposition in the brain and/or its effect on brain integrity and cognition. 3 Relation to clinical status, cognitive performances and brain volume 3.1 Relation to concomitant cognition and brain volume There have been quite numerous studies assessing the relation to cognition, even specifically within normal elderly, but the results remain overall puzzling: there are almost as many studies showing no significant relationships (Aizenstein et al., 2008; Marchant et al., 2012; Mormino et al., 2009; Resnick and Sojkova, 2011; Resnick et al., 2010; Sperling et al., 2009; Storandt et al., 2009; Storandt et al., 2012) as those showing a significant effect, and in the latter the relationship was rarely strong and general but rather modest and/or concerned a specific population with diverging results according to studies (Chételat et al., 2011; Kantarci et al., 2012; Mormino et al., 2009; Oh et al., in press; Oh et al., 2012; Pike et al., 2007; Rentz et al., 2011; Rodrigue et al., 2012). For example, relationships are usually reported with episodic memory deficits, but a study also reported a link with processing speed and working but not episodic memory (Rodrigue et al., 2012). Moreover, discrepant results have been reported in a same study with two CNE samples from two different databases (Mormino et al., 2009), and significant relationships have been observed only within females (Pike et al., 2011), non-ApoE4 carriers (Pike et al., 2011), or mainly in ApoE4 carriers (Kantarci et al., 2012) or low educated cognitively normal elderly (Rentz et al., 2010). In another study from the AIBL cohort, the relationships with episodic memory were found to concern only inferior temporal Aβ deposition (Chételat et al., 2011), or only normal elderly with subjective cognitive decline (Chételat et al., 2010a). Note that in the same cohort from the AIBL study, cognitively normal elderly without subjective cognitive decline showed a reverse relationship with higher memory performances in 11C-PIB-positive compared to 11C-PIB-negative cases (Chételat et al., 2010b). Similar findings have been reported in a previous preliminary study (Aizenstein et al., 2008). These 11C-PIB-positive “super-performers” also had larger temporal lobe, which suggests that they represent a particularly resistant subsample with larger brain reserve (Chételat et al., 2010b) (see also above for the effect of education and brain reserve). By contrast, in normal elderly with subjective memory decline, a relation was observed in the more expected direction with increased atrophy as amyloid load increases (Chételat et al., 2010a). In this study, the relationship was assessed voxel-to-voxel and local correlations were found in individuals with subjective cognitive decline within the posterior cingulate cortex and medial frontal area, which are the regions of highest Aβ deposition. There was no relationship within the hippocampus where atrophy predominates in AD, suggesting that atrophy is not due to local Aβ in this structure but involves other neuropathological processes. Distant (temporal) Aβ deposition for example has been found to be related to hippocampal atrophy (Bourgeat et al., 2010), and additional, partly independent, processes are thought to be involved (Chételat et al., 2011; La Joie et al., 2012). Neurofibrillary tangles are very likely to be implicated as these lesions develop very early in the hippocampus and they are known to correlate to neuronal loss and atrophy. When assessed in healthy elderly independently from whether or not they have subjective cognitive decline, findings were discrepant. Significant hippocampal atrophy has been reported in amyloid-positive cases in some studies (Hedden et al., 2009; Storandt et al., 2009), but not in others (Bourgeat et al., 2010; Dickerson et al., 2009), and temporal pole (Dickerson et al., 2009) or anterior and posterior cingulate cortex (Becker et al., 2011; Storandt et al., 2009) and prefrontal and lateral parietal cortex (Becker et al., 2011) atrophy or thickness reduction has been reported as well. When assessed linearly, a significant correlation has been found between global 11C-PIB and hippocampal atrophy in normal elderly (Mormino et al., 2009; Rowe et al., 2010), thought negative findings have been reported as well (Becker et al., 2011). Finally, a recent study reports a covariation between increase global 11C-PIB and decrease gray matter volume including in the medial and lateral temporal lobe, and medial frontal and posterior cingulate cortex (Oh et al., in press). As a whole, the relationships between cerebral Aβ deposits and concomitant cognitive performances or gray matter volume/thickness are complex and subtle. This probably reflects the fact that, if Aβ deposition has a role in neurodegeneration and cognitive deficits, it is probably indirect and/or blurred by the time decay between the different biomarkers (Jack et al., 2010), and/or by the intervention of other probably partly independent factors (e.g. tau-related changes, decreased metabolism, white matter abnormalities and disconnection, cognitive and brain compensation, etc.). There are accumulating evidences that Alzheimer's disease is a multifactorial disease with different and partly independent subtending processes rather than a single-process-driven pathology (Chételat et al., 2008; La Joie et al., 2012; Storandt et al., 2012); see http://www.alzforum.org/res/for/journal/detail.asp?liveID=199 for a live discussion on this topic. 3.2 Relation to prognosis — later changes in clinical status, cognition or brain volume Longitudinal studies assessing the relationships between baseline Aβ deposition and subsequent changes in cognition or brain volume usually report that the presence of Aβ deposition in the brain of cognitively normal elderly is associated with a worse prognosis. Thus, Villemagne et al. (2011) showed that 5 out of 32 (16%) of the 11C-PIB-positive cognitively normal elderly developed MCI or AD by 20 months and 8 out of 32 (25%) by 3 years while only one out of 73 11C-PIB-negative normal elderly developed MCI. Also, elevated Aβ deposition in cognitively normal elderly was shown to be related to greater clinical worsening (based on the CDR and/or ADAS-Cog scales) (Doraiswamy et al., 2012; Morris et al., 2009) and cognitive decline (in episodic and working memory and visuospatial ability) (Resnick et al., 2010; Storandt et al., 2009). In Doraiswamy et al. (2012), 23.5% of CDR0 amyloid-positive cognitively normal elderly converted to CDR0.5 within 18 months versus 5.5% within the amyloid-negative elderly. Finally, one longitudinal MRI study showed that 11C-PIB-positive cognitively normal elderly exhibited faster gray matter atrophy compared to 11C-PIB-negative cases at a group level (Chételat et al., 2012). Moreover, the amount of neocortical Aβ deposition correlated with the rate of subsequent atrophy in AD-sensitive brain areas (i.e. the temporal neocortex, hippocampus, posterior cingulate cortex, and angular gyrus), which was itself related to the rate of subsequent cognitive decline. These findings are consistent with a preliminary report in 13 healthy controls by Scheinin et al. (2009) or with findings in patients with MCI (Tosun et al., 2011a) (see the side article by Vandenberghe (2013--this issue)), as well as with studies showing that low CSF Aβ was associated with a faster rate of atrophy in similar AD-sensitive brain areas (Hua et al., 2010; Leow et al., 2009; Schott et al., 2010; Tosun et al., 2011b). It should be noted however that the findings in cognitively normal elderly should be considered carefully, keeping in mind that they were mostly obtained in community-recruited cohort studies where selection biases may be present, which may have an influence not only on the rate of amyloid-positive cases as discussed above, but also on the rate of conversion to AD and on the interaction between both factors (i.e. on the rate of conversion to AD of the amyloid-positive elderly). Consistent with this statement, the rate of conversion to AD in amyloid PET studies is usually particularly elevated, more than what would be expected given the incidence reported in the general population (see e.g. Whitwell et al., 2012). Although this questions the absolute number of converters within the amyloid-positives, these findings as a whole indicate that, on average, the prognosis in a group of individuals having Aβ in the brain, even if they are asymptomatic, is worse than that of a group of individuals with no Aβ. 4 The new research criteria for preclinical AD The considerable advances in neuroimaging and cerebrospinal biomarkers for AD in the last two decades, with amyloid imaging being the most recent and certainly the most notable of these developments, led to the revision of the NINCDS-ADRDA clinical diagnosis criteria for AD (McKhann et al., 1984). Several propositions have been published by different groups and addressing different clinical populations (Albert et al., 2011; Dubois et al., 2007; Dubois et al., 2010; McKhann et al., 2011; Sperling et al., 2011), and the present review will focus on the recommendation for the preclinical stages of AD (Sperling et al., 2011). These new criteria also take into account the hypothetical model of the chronology of the different biomarkers (Jack et al., 2010) itself largely based on the amyloid cascade hypothesis (Hardy and Selkoe, 2002), and consistently propose three stages in the preclinical phase: Aβ is present in the first stage without neuronal injury (stage 1), then neuronal injury is detected as well (stage 2), and then subtle cognitive decline appears (stage 3). When assessed in a population-based sample of 450 CNE, 43% of individuals were negative for the 3 biomarkers so they were considered as stage 0, and 16% were in stage 1, 12% in stage 2 and 3% in stage 3 (Jack et al., 2012). In addition, another category had to be added to account for the whole population, as 23% of subjects didn't fit into any group because they had AD-type neuronal injury (i.e. hippocampal atrophy and/or hypometabolism in the angular gyrus, posterior cingulate and inferior temporal cortex) without evidence of Aβ deposition. As this doesn't fit with the biomarkers chronology model that predicts that Aβ appears before neurodegeneration, these individuals were suspected to have non-AD pathology and were called as SNAP (for Suspected Non-Alzheimer's disease Pathophysiology). Longitudinal studies with a clinical follow-up of individuals in the different stages/categories are extremely important in the current context to confirm this view but also more generally to validate the diagnosis criteria and current dynamic biomarker models and further our understanding of the mechanisms underlying the disease. Actually, a recent publication provides first insights to these questions by showing the clinical outcome of participants according to each stage (Knopman et al., 2012). This study showed that the more positive biomarkers you have the more likely you are to convert, which confirms the usefulness of these biomarkers. It didn't allow to validate the chronology of biomarkers proposed by the model however, as the rate of conversion to MCI or dementia was similar in individuals in stage 1, i.e. who only had Aβ deposition in their brain (11%) as compared to the SNAP subjects, i.e. those having only neuronal injury but no Aβ (10%). This 10% conversion rate within the SNAP group was thus striking, but could still reflect the fact that they have non-AD related pathologies such as cerebrovascular disease. A recent publication however reveals that these so-called SNAP cases were indistinguishable from preclinical AD stages 1–3 on a variety of measures including those associated with the most frequent non-AD pathophysiological processes, i.e. cerebrovascular disease and α-synucleinopathy (Knopman et al., in press). The authors concluded that the initial appearance of brain injury biomarkers in cognitively normal elderly individuals may not depend on β-amyloidosis, which thus contradicts both the chronology proposed in the currently prevailing model and the amyloid cascade hypothesis. This, together with other arguments (e.g. Chételat, 2013; Fjell and Walhovd, 2012; Herrup, 2011) will probably further motivate researchers to consider alternatives to the amyloid hypothesis where Aβ promotes but is not necessarily responsible for, AD-related neurodegeneration (Chételat, 2013). 5 Longitudinal amyloid imaging As a whole, except in the first studies where sample sizes were relatively small and changes were not statistically significant (Jagust et al., 2010; Scheinin et al., 2009), longitudinal amyloid imaging studies showed significant increase in Aβ load in cognitively normal elderly of about 1% per year (Jack et al., 2009; Sojkova et al., 2011; Villain et al., 2012; Villemagne et al., 2011; Vlassenko et al., 2011). This increase was found to be higher in amyloid-positive than in negative cognitively normal elderly, and lower in cognitively normal elderly compared to MCI or AD though this was due to the fact that there were more amyloid-negative cases within the cognitively normal elderly than within the MCI or AD patients; when controlling for the 11C-PIB status, no difference was found in the rate of 11C-PIB accumulation between clinical groups (Villain et al., 2012). Most significant changes were observed in prefrontal, parietal, lateral temporal and occipital cortex (Sojkova et al., 2011; Villain et al., 2012) and anterior and posterior cingulate cortex (Sojkova et al., 2011). Increase in 11C-PIB over time in amyloid-negative cognitively normal elderly was found to be lower than in amyloid-positive but still significant. Individual analyses showed that there were more 11C-PIB accumulators (i.e. individuals showing significant 11C-PIB accumulation/increase over time) amongst 11C-PIB-positive (50%) than amongst 11C-PIB-negative (29%) cognitively normal elderly (Villain et al., 2012). The incidence of conversion from negative to positive within cognitively normal elderly was about 3% per year, and raised 7% in the ApoE4 carriers (Vlassenko et al., 2011). The rate of 11C-PIB accumulation remains higher in the 11C-PIB-positive cases when only considering the accumulators, suggesting that those with higher 11C-PIB have greater rate of 11C-PIB accumulation, while this trend tends to reverse in those with high baseline 11C-PIB retention, consistent with the concept of a saturable process of Aβ deposition as the 11C-PIB retention reaches highest values (Villain et al., 2012). Further discussion on the dynamic of Aβ all over the course of the disease including in clinical stages will be provided in the side review by Vandenberghe (2013--this issue). 6 Ethical considerations The progressive discovery of biomarkers for AD that peaks with amyloid neuroimaging, their use in the new proposed criteria for AD including specifically for preclinical AD, the recent approval of Amyvid (florbetapir F18 injection) by the FDA on April 9th 2012, altogether revive the debate on ethical challenges of preclinical AD that has been already, at least partly, addressed with the development of ApoE genotyping and predictive genetic testing. There have been an increasing interest in this question recently, and several groups develop specific studies and publish reviews fully-dedicated to this issue (Blennow and Zetterberg, 2010; Draper et al., 2010; Karlawish, 2011; Mattsson et al., 2010; Prvulovic and Hampel, 2011; Roberts and Tersegno, 2010). The present review was not aimed at providing a detailed overview on ethical and social issues associated with preclinical AD. However, inspired by these authors, the main questions will be highlighted as they are crucial when dealing with amyloid imaging in preclinical populations. Thus, early diagnosis in general, amyloid imaging in preclinical population in particular, raises important ethical issues as regard to disclosure of these information to individuals. There is a distinction between clinical assessments versus research. Researchers have no obligation to disclose biomarker results to participants, and the informed consent explains to them why they will not be given such information (Karlawish, 2011). As for the clinic, we are far from a routine use in clinical practice for the preclinical diagnosis for AD: amyloid imaging doesn't fulfill the requirements for a screening test (in terms of cost, accuracy, availability, etc) according to the principles and practice of screening for disease published by the World Health Organization (Wilson, 1968). FDA approval is only for cognitively impaired patients, and the use of biomarkers in preclinical AD is only for research. However, scientists and clinicians should prepare to face the problem, notably to anticipate the hopeful future development of disease-modifying treatments. While it is quite clear that there are amyloid-positive cases amongst cognitively normal elderly, and that their risk of conversion to AD is probably higher than for amyloid-negative cognitively normal elderly, it is also clear that not all amyloid-positive cognitively normal elderly will convert to AD at least in the following couple of years. Thus, the rate of conversion to MCI/AD in amyloid-positive cognitively normal elderly is about 15–25% within the following 2–3 years (see above), which means that about 80% will remain stable over this period. The AIBL study offers one of the largest database with amyloid PET imaging and with the longest follow-up time, and it shows that some 11C-PIB-positive cognitively normal elderly remain cognitively stable even after a 6 year follow-up (Rowe and Villemagne, personal communication). This leads to the first following question: is it ethical to deliver an amyloid-scan result while not all amyloid-positive individuals will develop AD. This means that what is delivered is not diagnosis but risk information. This distinction is very important as it should be perfectly clear, for the clinician of course but also for the patient and his family, that what is disclosed from an amyloid scan is information about the presence of Aβ deposition in the brain, associated with a risk to develop AD, but not on the diagnosis of AD itself. This is thus the same situation as for disclosing ApoE genotype and scientists thus take their inspiration from the relatively abundant literacy on disclosing genetic information. This leads to a second question that more generally applies to early AD diagnosis: is it ethical to deliver the risk information related to an amyloid-scan result while there is no treatment? The growing distance between scientific advances in terms of diagnosis versus treatment and the uncoupling between the diagnosis and the clinical expression of the disease also raise ethical issues. When trying to answer to these questions, one should also take into account patient's right to know and find the balance between the patient's desire to know his risk developing AD and the clinician's desire to mitigate the potential harm of that information. These are very difficult questions to answer, as of course there are both advantages and disadvantages in disclosing risk information such as the results of an amyloid scan in asymptomatic individuals and in preclinical AD diagnosis (see Table 2 for examples of advantages and disadvantages). Our advances in terms of preclinical diagnosis and biomarkers should thus be paralleled by evidence-based advances in our knowledge on the way to disclose this information and on its psychological implications, as well as by societal and legislative evolution. More specifically, studies are needed (and are currently under progress) to track the emotional and physical impact of the disclosure, and to develop and disseminate best practice guide on how to disclose the result of an amyloid scan. Again, such procedures have already been defined for disclosure of genetic information (such as providing time for reflection prior to disclosing results, psychological support, delivery in a face-to-face meeting, etc) that provide a significant basis for adaptation to the case of amyloid imaging in preclinical populations. We cannot work on amyloid imaging in preclinical AD without anticipating the related ethical challenges. Clearly, we are not ready yet for the diagnosis of preclinical AD. There are numerous challenges that should first be faced, several essential questions of ethical implications that still need to be answered; our knowledge on how patients actually react to early diagnosis is still too scarce and preliminary steps are thus needed to translate research results into clinical practice and policy. 7 Conclusion As a whole, there are evidences for which there is absolutely no doubt on: some cognitively normal elderly have Aβ deposition in their brain, the prevalence of amyloid-positive cases increases in at-risk populations, the prognosis for these individuals (as a group) is worse than for those with no Aβ deposition, and significant increase in Aβ deposition over time is detectable in cognitively normal elderly. Other points are more obscure: the relation between Aβ deposition and AD-related changes (cognition, atrophy, hypometabolism and connectivity) is complex, the sequence and cause-to-effect relationships between the different biomarkers are challenged, and the individual outcome associated with an amyloid-positive scan is still unknown: will all amyloid-positive elderly eventually develop AD and when? Further studies are needed to know how to translate group findings into individual use, i.e. how to use amyloid imaging to support AD diagnosis in preclinical individuals. Preclinical amyloid imaging also raises important ethical issues. There is a distinction between clinic and research for the use of amyloid imaging, and between clinicians and researchers for the disclosure of information. Amyloid imaging is definitively useful to understand the role of Aβ in early stages, to define at-risk populations for research or for clinical trial, and to assess the effects of anti-amyloid treatments. However, we are not ready yet to translate research results into clinical practice and policy. The considerable advances of research in terms of amyloid imaging, biomarkers and preclinical diagnosis over the last decade should be paralleled by significant progress in our knowledge on the way of disclosing such information and its impact, as well as societal and legislation adaptation to anticipate the future where preclinical diagnosis and disease-modifying treatment will hopefully be available.
            Bookmark
            • Record: found
            • Abstract: found
            • Article: not found

            Genetic counseling and testing for Alzheimer disease: joint practice guidelines of the American College of Medical Genetics and the National Society of Genetic Counselors.

            Alzheimer disease is the most common cause of dementia. It occurs worldwide and affects all ethnic groups. The incidence of Alzheimer disease is increasing due, in part, to increased life expectancy and the aging baby boomer generation. The average lifetime risk of developing Alzheimer disease is 10-12%. This risk at least doubles with the presence of a first-degree relative with the disorder. Despite its limited utility, patients express concern over their risk and, in some instances, request testing. Furthermore, research has demonstrated that testing individuals for apolipoprotein E can be valuable and safe in certain contexts. However, because of the complicated genetic nature of the disorder, few clinicians are prepared to address the genetic risks of Alzheimer disease with their patients. Given the increased awareness in family history thanks to family history campaigns, the increasing incidence of Alzheimer disease, and the availability of direct to consumer testing, patient requests for information is increasing. This practice guideline provides clinicians with a framework for assessing their patients' genetic risk for Alzheimer disease, identifying which individuals may benefit from genetic testing, and providing the key elements of genetic counseling for AD.
              Bookmark
              • Record: found
              • Abstract: found
              • Article: not found

              Survival times in people with dementia: analysis from population based cohort study with 14 year follow-up.

              To provide estimates of survival after onset of dementia by age, sex, self reported health, disability, and severity of cognitive impairment. Analysis of participants from prospective population based cohort study in 1991-2003, with follow-up of dementia status in all individuals after two and six years (in one centre) and 10 years and in subsamples additionally at six and eight years and mortality until 2005. Multicentre population based study in England and Wales: two rural and three urban centres. 438 participants who developed dementia from a population based study of 13 004 individuals aged 65 years and over drawn from primary care population registers. Sociodemographic factors, cognitive function, specific health conditions, and self reported health collected at each interview. Cox's proportional hazards regression models were used to identify predictors of mortality from the selected variables in people who received diagnosis of dementia according the study's criteria. By December 2005, 356 of the 438 (81%) participants who developed dementia during the study had died. Estimated median survival time from onset of dementia to death was 4.1 years (interquartile range 2.5-7.6) for men and 4.6 years (2.9-7.0) for women. There was a difference of nearly seven years in survival between the younger old and the oldest people with dementia: 10.7 (25th centile 5.6) for ages 65-69; 5.4 (interquartile range 3.4-8.3) for ages 70-79; 4.3 (2.8-7.0) for ages 80-89, and 3.8 (2.3-5.2) years for ages > or =90. Significant factors that predicted mortality in the presence of dementia during the follow-up included sex, age of onset, and disability. These analyses give a population based estimated median survival for incident dementia of 4.5 years. Such estimates can be used for prognosis and planning for patients, carers, service providers, and policy makers.
                Bookmark

                Author and article information

                Contributors
                Role: Handling Associate Editor
                Role: Ethics Editor
                Journal
                J Alzheimers Dis
                J. Alzheimers Dis
                JAD
                Journal of Alzheimer's Disease
                IOS Press (Nieuwe Hemweg 6B, 1013 BG Amsterdam, The Netherlands )
                1387-2877
                1875-8908
                20 February 2018
                2018
                : 62
                : 2
                : 487-498
                Affiliations
                [a ]Department of Public Health and Primary Care, Institute of Public Health, University of Cambridge School of Clinical Medicine , Cambridge, UK
                [b ]Department of Medical Ethics and Philosophy of Medicine, Erasmus MC, University Medical Center Rotterdam , Rotterdam, The Netherlands
                [c ]Alzheimer Europe, Luxembourg
                [d ]Department of Neurology, Radboud University Medical Center , Nijmegen, The Netherlands
                [e ]BarcelonaBeta Brain Research Centre , Fundació Pasqual Maragall, Barcelona, Spain
                [f ]Centre of Mental Health, Imperial College London , London, UK
                [g ]Centre for Clinical Brain Sciences, University of Edinburgh , Edinburgh, UK
                Author notes
                [* ]Correspondence to: Dr. Richard Milne, Department of Public Health and Primary Care, Institute of Public Health, Forvie Site, University of Cambridge School of Clinical Medicine, Box 113 Cambridge Biomedical Campus, Cambridge CB2 0SP, UK. Tel.: +44 01223 761912; E-mail: rjm231@ 123456cam.ac.uk .
                Article
                JAD170813
                10.3233/JAD-170813
                5836405
                29480179
                9bac3f18-3622-4136-8e11-60dad9ffae74
                © 2018 – IOS Press and the authors. All rights reserved

                This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Non-Commercial (CC BY-NC 4.0) License, which permits unrestricted non-commercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

                History
                : 7 December 2017
                Categories
                Ethics Review

                biomarkers,disclosure,focus groups,ethics,qualitative research

                Comments

                Comment on this article