15
views
0
recommends
+1 Recommend
0 collections
    0
    shares
      • Record: found
      • Abstract: found
      • Article: found
      Is Open Access

      Reply to comments by Olson et al. 2017 and Stien 2017

      reply
      1 , 2
      Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences
      The Royal Society

      Read this article at

      Bookmark
          There is no author summary for this article yet. Authors can add summaries to their articles on ScienceOpen to make them more accessible to a non-specialist audience.

          Abstract

          The management of large carnivores remains a contentious issue in many countries. Among the most contentious management options is ‘tolerance hunting’, or the killing of predators to increase tolerance among groups of people who do not accept the presence of these animals [1,2]. In [3,4], we used Bayesian state space models to evaluate the hypothesis that liberalizing culling of wolves changed wolf population dynamics from 1995 to 2012, and concluded it slowed growth, which we inferred was owing to increased poaching. Olson et al. [5] and Stien [6] re-visit our paper and we address their criticisms below. First, we disagree with Olson et al.'s [5] and Stien's [6] assertions that our paper ignores the literature or reports it in a biased manner. We simply disagree about the interpretation of the literature as we explain below. While they can have a different interpretation of those papers, it does not mean that ours is incorrect and Stien's [6, p. 1] phrasing ‘biased reporting of previously published results’ almost suggests intent from us to mislead the reader. Both Olson et al. [5] and Stien [6] raised the issue of density dependence analysed by Stenglein et al. [7]. In that paper, the information on density dependence relevant to our paper is in figures 3, S2.4, S2.5 and S2.6 (we cannot find reported numerical estimates on how recruitment changed during the relevant period for our study in [7]). Stenglein et al. [7, p. 5] wrote that ‘The evidence for a negative slope of the line for t > 18 was 69.0% (proportion of posterior that was <0)’ but this concerns all years post-1998, which also include many years without culling. For the relevant period for our paper (when culling was allowed or wolf years 2004–2012), we need to interpret the figures ourselves. On figures 3, S2.4, S2.5 and S2.6 in [7], we find no obvious difference between the confidence intervals of annual recruitment estimates. In fact, the only significant drop in recruitment seems to happen much earlier, at the beginning of the t > 18 period (1998–2001 approximately) whereas the years with culling seem to show a stable recruitment regardless of the models used [7]. Because Stenglein et al. [7] clearly concluded that they found no density dependence on survival, we observed then and still interpret Stenglein et al. [7] to show no density dependence for the period relevant to our study. An additional sentence in our discussion in [3] explaining what we just explained above might have been welcome but seemed a digression. We also chose not to mention that Stenglein et al. [7, p. 5] appear to trust their model because ‘48.4% of the time, the estimated population sizes in Wisconsin from 1981 to 2011 were within the 95% posterior intervals of μt ’ implying that more than half the time their estimates failed this relatively undemanding test. Stenglein et al. [7] also did not, in our opinion, properly handle uncertainty by using the midpoint between minimum and maximum population size as their population count (while we allowed fluctuations between minimum and maximum in [3]). Both Olson et al. [5] and Stien [6] further insist that the decline in growth rate is owing to negative density dependence. Olson et al. [5] present a compilation of studies, but which also includes some unrelated to negative density dependence (see our electronic supplementary material). Neither of those papers present, in our opinion, empirical evidence to support a mechanism for density dependence in the population and period under discussion. Stien [6] argues that the quadratic relationship he found for area against population size is evidence of negative density dependence. However, as we wrote previously [8], one must first demonstrate a mechanism to assert negative density dependence. Indeed, the United States Fish and Wildlife Service reported that the Wisconsin wolf population grew from minima of 746 to 866 by April 2016 [9] after all wolf-killing including tolerance hunting was barred in December 2014, or a 1-year growth of 16%, which is larger than the annual median growth during our study period. This accelerating growth at the relevant population size demonstrates that there is still no evidence consistent with negative density dependence in the Wisconsin wolf population during the period of interest for our study. Olson et al. [5] also argue that their previous study [10] demonstrated that illegal killing decreases with increasing availability of lethal management. However, this study [10] was, in our opinion and that of an anonymous reviewer, not quantitatively rigorous. One reviewer of our paper [3] indeed agreed and wrote that our ‘paper is also important because the results are at least somewhat contradictory to a recent paper Olson et al. [10]. That recent paper had some important shortcoming for which this paper seems to “fix”’. We admit we might have explained the below shortcomings in our original paper [3] but did not wish to appear confrontational. Olson et al. [10] assumed that observed poaching correlated tightly to unobserved poaching (even for radio-collared wolves). Embracing this assumption leads to the faulty conclusion that observed poaching is an unbiased sample of all poaching and can be used as the response variable for a correlation with temporal changes in policy. Treves et al. [11] did not find support for that assumption. In a separate study in Scandinavia, Liberg et al. [12] found that two thirds of poaching was not observed. For Wisconsin wolves, Treves et al. [11] estimated that same observation error to be half of all poached wolves. Olson et al. [10] also used the number of recovered radio-collared wolves inferred to have died from poaching as their response variable, without considering errors in inferring poaching as a cause of death. Systematic errors in attributing poaching to Wisconsin wolf carcasses ranged from 6–37% depending on which subsample one examined, as reported by veterinary pathologists contributing to Treves et al. [13]. Both Olson et al. [10] and Treves et al. [11,13] agree that a high proportion of radio-collared wolves disappeared without trace (unknown fate), which must be addressed in some way in any analysis of poaching [11]. Most importantly, Olson et al. [10] ignored exposure time of radio-collared wolves. We do not understand why they did not use a survival (time to event) model with the proportion of the year with culling as an explanatory variable. However, even using a time to event model would require a proper treatment of unknown fates. Finally, Olson et al. [10] did not seem to consider that marked animals (radio-collared wolves) may not suffer the same mortality pattern as the unmarked population. This has been shown specifically in two recent studies of wolves, which have undermined the assumption of identical mortality patterns [14,15]. Olson et al. [5] and Stien [6] raise other points which we address in detail in our electronic supplementary material. Briefly, Stien [6] claims that there is a strong link between probability of reproduction and proportion of the year with legal culling. However, we believe other models in Stien [6] supplementary code do not support this conclusion, which, if they would, would still not warrant a change of our conclusions (see electronic supplementary material). We explain Olson et al. [5]'s assertion—that our hypothesis is not parsimonious—is built on a misunderstanding of the cause-and-effect relationships between cognition and behaviour. Moreover, Olson et al. [5]'s hypothesis of density dependence is not supported by evidence (see above), so its simplicity does not give it strength. We also argue that there is no support for the frustration hypothesis proposed by Olson et al. [5] because previous research demonstrates that tolerance for wolves declined, and inclination to poach rose, in the years following culling authority. Here and elsewhere, the reasoning in Olson et al. [5] leaves the impression of cherry-picking the literature while accusing us inaccurately of ignoring or misrepresenting it. Olson et al. [5] insinuate that we chose to start our analysis in 1995 because it somehow supported our hypothesis. Our choice is justified by two of Olson et al.'s [5] co-authors writing how monitoring substantially improved after 1995 [16]. The papers they cite [7,17,18] that begin analyses earlier do not seem to account for that change in census methods, which may affect their results. Finally, Olson et al. [5] criticize us for calling our study ‘quasi experimental’ and write that it is instead a ‘worst case design’ despite having published on the exact same study system [10]. We do not follow the logic by which a system can suddenly become the worst when other different authors write about it. Overall, the pattern emerging from analyses in Olson et al. [5,10] is one of a stream of unrigorous assertions which together portray a picture of the Wisconsin wolf population that is inaccurate. When management policies are built on such weak assertions, these policies cannot have a scientific basis, as has been shown for wolf hunting in the United States [19]. In addition, Olson et al. [5] seem, in our opinion, inclined to divert from a collegial discussion and adopt the language and style of advocacy. While there may be many reasons to pledge allegiance to management agencies, we believe that scholarly debates are not compatible with ad-hominem attacks and misleading soundbites. We appreciate the scrutiny that our analysis and our writing have sparked. Science progresses through invalidation of hypotheses and presentation of new evidence, therefore we welcome scrutiny of our work and collegial discussions. However, we also feel obligated to point out that statements supporting the tolerance hunting hypothesis, either from scientists or governments, seem to be taken for granted and evade scrutiny. A recent illustration is a paper about wolves in Norway bluntly claiming that ‘it is not an unreasonable expectation that allowing legal harvest might prevent some of the illegal killing’ [20, p. 135]. In our opinion, the careful wording of nuances in the above sentence only signals a value-based statement intended to influence policy regardless of evidence. While our model has faced substantial and legitimate scrutiny, scientists have remained silent about flaws or lack of evidence supporting the tolerance hunting hypothesis. In other words, killing predators appears immune to evidence-based scrutiny, while not killing predators must be justified by the highest level of evidence. One possible reason is that killing predators may simply be viewed as not worthy of justification unless one is driven by emotions [21], an attitude revealing contempt for changing public attitudes about the value of wildlife [22] and a refusal to serve the broad public interest [23]. Another possible reason may be that killing predators is a goal by itself regardless of its effectiveness in reducing poaching because it provides political services [24]. As a consequence, tolerance hunting is today a widespread management intervention for large carnivores [2] (see our electronic supplementary material for updated context), perhaps because it has the potential to justify large scale killing and is extremely difficult to evaluate scientifically. We believe that double standards in evaluating evidence are hazardous. The double standard that we observe runs contrary to the precautionary principle and the level of scrutiny should not be lower or plainly absent for writings supporting tolerance hunting than for results invalidating it. We conclude by hoping that the debate our paper triggered will encourage further research on this controversial topic. Supplementary Material Supplementary Material

          Related collections

          Most cited references22

          • Record: found
          • Abstract: not found
          • Article: not found

          Hunting for large carnivore conservation

            Bookmark
            • Record: found
            • Abstract: found
            • Article: found
            Is Open Access

            Blood does not buy goodwill: allowing culling increases poaching of a large carnivore

            Quantifying environmental crime and the effectiveness of policy interventions is difficult because perpetrators typically conceal evidence. To prevent illegal uses of natural resources, such as poaching endangered species, governments have advocated granting policy flexibility to local authorities by liberalizing culling or hunting of large carnivores. We present the first quantitative evaluation of the hypothesis that liberalizing culling will reduce poaching and improve population status of an endangered carnivore. We show that allowing wolf (Canis lupus) culling was substantially more likely to increase poaching than reduce it. Replicated, quasi-experimental changes in wolf policies in Wisconsin and Michigan, USA, revealed that a repeated policy signal to allow state culling triggered repeated slowdowns in wolf population growth, irrespective of the policy implementation measured as the number of wolves killed. The most likely explanation for these slowdowns was poaching and alternative explanations found no support. When the government kills a protected species, the perceived value of each individual of that species may decline; so liberalizing wolf culling may have sent a negative message about the value of wolves or acceptability of poaching. Our results suggest that granting management flexibility for endangered species to address illegal behaviour may instead promote such behaviour.
              Bookmark
              • Record: found
              • Abstract: found
              • Article: found
              Is Open Access

              Predators and the public trust

              ABSTRACT Many democratic governments recognize a duty to conserve environmental resources, including wild animals, as a public trust for current and future citizens. These public trust principles have informed two centuries of U.S.A. Supreme Court decisions and environmental laws worldwide. Nevertheless numerous populations of large‐bodied, mammalian carnivores (predators) were eradicated in the 20th century. Environmental movements and strict legal protections have fostered predator recoveries across the U.S.A. and Europe since the 1970s. Now subnational jurisdictions are regaining management authority from central governments for their predator subpopulations. Will the history of local eradication repeat or will these jurisdictions adopt public trust thinking and their obligation to broad public interests over narrower ones? We review the role of public trust principles in the restoration and preservation of controversial species. In so doing we argue for the essential roles of scientists from many disciplines concerned with biological diversity and its conservation. We look beyond species endangerment to future generations' interests in sustainability, particularly non‐consumptive uses. Although our conclusions apply to all wild organisms, we focus on predators because of the particular challenges they pose for government trustees, trust managers, and society. Gray wolves Canis lupus L. deserve particular attention, because detailed information and abundant policy debates across regions have exposed four important challenges for preserving predators in the face of interest group hostility. One challenge is uncertainty and varied interpretations about public trustees' responsibilities for wildlife, which have created a mosaic of policies across jurisdictions. We explore how such mosaics have merits and drawbacks for biodiversity. The other three challenges to conserving wildlife as public trust assets are illuminated by the biology of predators and the interacting behavioural ecologies of humans and predators. The scientific community has not reached consensus on sustainable levels of human‐caused mortality for many predator populations. This challenge includes both genuine conceptual uncertainty and exploitation of scientific debate for political gain. Second, human intolerance for predators exposes value conflicts about preferences for some wildlife over others and balancing majority rule with the protection of minorities in a democracy. We examine how differences between traditional assumptions and scientific studies of interactions between people and predators impede evidence‐based policy. Even if the prior challenges can be overcome, well‐reasoned policy on wild animals faces a greater challenge than other environmental assets because animals and humans change behaviour in response to each other in the short term. These coupled, dynamic responses exacerbate clashes between uses that deplete wildlife and uses that enhance or preserve wildlife. Viewed in this way, environmental assets demand sophisticated, careful accounting by disinterested trustees who can both understand the multidisciplinary scientific measurements of relative costs and benefits among competing uses, and justly balance the needs of all beneficiaries including future generations. Without public trust principles, future trustees will seldom prevail against narrow, powerful, and undemocratic interests. Without conservation informed by public trust thinking predator populations will face repeated cycles of eradication and recovery. Our conclusions have implications for the many subfields of the biological sciences that address environmental trust assets from the atmosphere to aquifers.
                Bookmark

                Author and article information

                Journal
                Proc Biol Sci
                Proc. Biol. Sci
                RSPB
                royprsb
                Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences
                The Royal Society
                0962-8452
                1471-2954
                29 November 2017
                22 November 2017
                22 November 2017
                : 284
                : 1867
                : 20171743
                Affiliations
                [1 ]Grimsö Wildlife Research Station, Department of Ecology, Swedish University of Agricultural Sciences , 73091 Riddarhyttan, Sweden
                [2 ]Nelson Institute for Environmental Studies, University of Wisconsin , 30A Science Hall, 550 North Park Street, Madison, WI 53706, USA
                Author notes
                [†]

                These authors contributed equally to this study.

                Electronic supplementary material is available online at https://dx.doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.c.3931933.

                Author information
                http://orcid.org/0000-0002-6727-1070
                Article
                rspb20171743
                10.1098/rspb.2017.1743
                5719172
                29167362
                9e89f598-3cac-4d2e-a5f1-25ea72639193
                © 2017 The Authors.

                Published by the Royal Society under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/, which permits unrestricted use, provided the original author and source are credited.

                History
                : 3 August 2017
                : 20 October 2017
                Categories
                1001
                60
                Ecology
                Invited Reply
                Custom metadata
                November 29, 2017

                Life sciences
                Life sciences

                Comments

                Comment on this article