21
views
0
recommends
+1 Recommend
0 collections
    0
    shares
      • Record: found
      • Abstract: found
      • Article: found
      Is Open Access

      Patterns of motor activity in spontaneously hypertensive rats compared to Wistar Kyoto rats

      research-article

      Read this article at

      Bookmark
          There is no author summary for this article yet. Authors can add summaries to their articles on ScienceOpen to make them more accessible to a non-specialist audience.

          Abstract

          Background

          Increased motor activity is a defining characteristic of patients with ADHD, and spontaneously hypertensive rats have been suggested to be an animal model of this disorder. In the present study, we wanted to use linear and non-linear methods to explore differences in motor activity patterns in SHR/NCrl rats compared to Wistar Kyoto (WKY/NHsd) rats.

          Methods

          A total number of 42 rats (23 SHR/NCrl and 19 WKY/NHsd, male and female) were tested. At PND 51, the animals’ movements were video-recorded during an operant test procedure that lasted 90 min. Total activity level and velocity (mean and maximum), standard deviation (SD) and root mean square successive differences (RMSSD) were calculated. In addition, we used Fourier analysis, autocorrelations and two measures of complexity to characterize the time series; sample entropy and symbolic dynamics.

          Results

          The SHR/NCrl rats showed increased total activity levels in addition to increased mean and maximum velocity of movements. The variability measures, SD and RMSSD, were markedly lower in the SHR/NCrl compared to the WKY/NHsd rats. At the same time, the SHR/NCrl rats displayed a higher complexity of the time series, particularly with regard to the total activity level as evidenced by analyses of sample entropy and symbolic dynamics. Autocorrelation analyses also showed differences between the two strains. In the Fourier analysis, the SHR/NCrl rats had an increased variance in the high frequency part of the spectrum, corresponding to the time period of 9–17 s.

          Conclusion

          The findings show that in addition to increased total activity and velocity of movement, the organization of behavior is different in SHR/NCrl relative to WKY/NHsd controls. Compared to controls, behavioral variability is reduced in SHR/NCrl at an aggregate level, and, concomitantly, more complex and unpredictable from moment-to-moment. These finding emphasize the importance of the measures and methods used when characterizing behavioral variability. If valid for ADHD, the results indicate that decreased behavioral variability can co-exist with increased behavioral complexity, thus representing a challenge to current theories of variability in ADHD.

          Related collections

          Most cited references56

          • Record: found
          • Abstract: found
          • Article: not found

          Reaction time variability in ADHD: a meta-analytic review of 319 studies.

          Individuals with ADHD are characterized as ubiquitously slower and more variable than their unaffected peers, and increased reaction time (RT) variability is considered by many to reflect an etiologically important characteristic of ADHD. The present review critically evaluates these claims through meta-analysis of 319 studies of RT variability in children, adolescents, and adults with ADHD relative to typically developing (TD) groups, clinical control groups, and themselves (subtype comparisons, treatment and motivation effects). Random effects models corrected for measurement unreliability and publication bias revealed that children/adolescents (Hedges' g=0.76) and adults (g=0.46) with ADHD demonstrated greater RT variability relative to TD groups. This increased variability was attenuated by psychostimulant treatment (g=-0.74), but unaffected by non-stimulant medical and psychosocial interventions. Individuals with ADHD did not evince slower processing speed (mean RT) after accounting for RT variability, whereas large magnitude RT variability deficits remained after accounting for mean RT. Adolescents and adults with ADHD were indistinguishable from clinical control groups, and children with ADHD were only minimally more variable than clinical control children (g=0.25). Collectively, results of the meta-analysis indicate that RT variability reflects a stable feature of ADHD and other clinical disorders that is robust to systematic differences across studies. Copyright © 2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
            Bookmark
            • Record: found
            • Abstract: not found
            • Article: not found

            Non-linear dynamics for clinicians: chaos theory, fractals, and complexity at the bedside.

              Bookmark
              • Record: found
              • Abstract: found
              • Article: not found

              Increased Brain Signal Variability Accompanies Lower Behavioral Variability in Development

              Introduction During neurodevelopment, behavioural performance tends to improve in terms of speed and accuracy. This improvement usually entails a decrease in trial-to-trial variability as performance approaches ceiling e.g. [1],[2]. A fundamental question is whether the neuronal dynamics that mediate behaviour show a similar decrease in variability. There are two arguments that furnish opposite predictions. The first is that neurodevelopmental trajectories will converge on optimal structure and dynamics, suggesting that trial-to-trial variations in evoked neuronal responses will decrease with age. This argument is particularly attractive in view of known neural pruning that accompanies normal brain maturation [3]. The second argument is that, if the computations underlying performance rest on adaptive, metastable brain dynamics [4], there may be an age-related increase in trial-to-trial variability. Functional variability represents a greater repertoire of metastable brain states and the more facile state transitions [5]. In this work, we disambiguated between these competing hypotheses by relating trial-to-trial variability in behavior with brain electrical activity across a sample of children and adults. Neural systems can show a somewhat counterintuitive property, whereby optimal operations occur in the presence of a moderate amount of internal variability or noise [6]. For example, the phenomena of stochastic resonance describes how a simple nonlinear system can show an optimal signal-to-noise ratio with a moderate amount of noise, which enables the detection of weak periodic signals [7]–[9]. From cell channels to synapse to neural ensembles, noise seems to be an important parameter that shapes responsivity [10],[11]. It has been suggested that there may need to be a degree of physiological variability for the brain to adapt effectively to an uncertain environment [12]. While there are sophisticated computational models demonstrating the beneficial effects of noise for network dynamics [13],[14], the direct relation between neural variability and the behavior variability of the organism has been largely unexplored [15]–[17]. We sought to characterize the relation of neurophysiological variability and behavioral variability in maturation. Critically, our focus was not on variability of the signal across individuals within a group (interindividual), but rather the single-trial variability within an individual (intraindividual) [18]. Measures of single trial variability in electroencephalographic (EEG) signals within subjects were related to variability in response latency and accuracy. EEG signals were measured from children (ages 8–15 yrs, n = 55) and young adults (20–33 yrs, n = 24) during the performance of a face recognition task [19]. Results Behaviorally, all age groups showed high accuracy in the task, with adults near ceiling (Figure 1a). Recognition accuracy for children, while lower than for adults (one-way ANOVA, F (4, 74),  = 9.07, P 10 Hz). The average stimulus evoked electrical potentials across the age groups (Figure 1b) showed a characteristic maturational change with greater amplitude but longer latency for a positive deflection peaking at about 100 ms post-stimulus (P100)[20],[21]. A second observation was that the subsequent deflections (N100, P200) are easily observable in adults and gradually emerge in children [19]. The bias in children towards higher amplitude, but slower, electrophysiological signals is paralleled by a differential distribution in prestimulus, or baseline, spectral power. Across age, there was a gradual reduction in low frequency spectral power and a relative increase in power at higher frequencies (Figure 1c, see supplementary material Text S1 for statistical analysis and Figures S1). The relative change in spectral power density presumably underlies the reduction in the latency of the evoked responses and its multicomponent nature [22],[23], where the lower frequency bias in children would yield slow and broad evoked potentials. The emergence of higher frequencies with maturation would both decrease the evoked response latency and allow additional deflections to emerge (i.e., N100, P200, etc.). Two measures were used to evaluate brain signal variability. First, principal components analysis (PCA) was performed within each subject on their single trial EEG recordings. PCA identified the number of orthogonal dimensions, expressed as a proportion of the total possible, needed to express a certain amount of trial-to-trial variability (90% in the present case) for each channel. In a deterministic system with highly stereotyped responses, only a few dimensions are needed to capture most of the variability. To the extent that trial-to-trial recordings differ from one another, total variability increases, and hence PCA dimensionality increases. PCA dimensionality estimates for the 200-ms intervals pre- and post-stimulus onset increased across the age groups (Fig 2, a, b). Statistical analysis of PCA dimensionality with partial least squares [PLS, 24] confirmed a significant linear increase across age groups in dimensionality that was expressed stably across most EEG channels. In other words, adults showed the most variability in the measured brain signals. 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000106.g002 Figure 2 Within-subject brain variability measures across age groups. A) shows group mean results across the scalp for pre- and post-stimulus PCA dimensionality estimate of trial-to-trial variability (top two rows) and MSE area under the curve (bottom row). Scalp maps of group means were obtained by interpolating values from single channel group mean values. Gradual increase in all three brain variability measures across age groups is evident. PLS statistical analysis of each measure detected a pattern of linear increase, which was significant (p 3.5) for most channels. B) shows group means for MSE estimates across temporal scales for channel O2, together with corresponding standard errors. Similar entropy curves were obtained for all channels and showed maturation-related increase in entropy at all scales. Given consistent age-related differences at all time scales, the area under the MSE curve was taken as a summary measure of maturational changes in entropy (e.g., (A), bottom row). A second aspect of signal variability is its temporal predictability, which can be measured using Multiscale Entropy [MSE 25]. MSE measures sample entropy [26],[27] of the signal at successively downsampled time series, with a scale of 1 being the original time series and scale t indicating a time series created by averaging t adjacent points. MSE assigns low values to both highly deterministic and completely random signals, making it an explicit measure of signal complexity [28]. MSE estimation applied to single trial data for each channel showed that sample entropy measures were highest for adults across all temporal scales and lowest for the youngest children, with the intermediate age groups falling along an ordinal trend (Fig 2b). Given that consistent age differences were observed at all time scales, the area under the MSE curve was computed for each subject to compare age-related differences. Multivariate statistical analysis with PLS confirmed a significant age-related increase in MSE that was expressed stably across most of the EEG channels (Fig 2a). Taken together, the PCA and MSE measures indicate that, contrary to behavioral variability, brain variability increases with maturation. In some ways, this could be deduced from the group differences in the relative spectral density distribution, wherein one may expect signals that are dominated by low frequencies to show less variability than those with relatively stronger contributions from higher frequencies. For example, by increasing the relative magnitude of the Fourier coefficients for low frequencies in the adult EEG data, it is possible to get PCA dimensionality and MSE estimates similar to children (Figure S2). However, spectral density and variability are not completely interdependent, because jittering the phase of the Fourier coefficients, while maintaining their relative magnitude, has no impact on spectral density, but changes PCA dimensionality and MSE estimates (Figure S3). This is because PCA and MSE are sensitive to the dependencies within the signals that do not affect spectral density. Such sensitivities likely reflect transients in neural processing, and would be most evident in a system with enhanced capacity for signal processing and complexity. The final and most important part of this investigation was to relate behavioral variability, brain variability, and maturation. We addressed these issues using PLS to analyze the correlations between our measures of dynamical variability (pre and post-PCA dimensionality and MSE) and their phenotypic correlates (RT-variability, accuracy, and age). We also include mean RT in the analysis to determine whether the correlation patterns we observed were specific to behavioral measures of variability, or to any metric showing a maturational change. Figure 3 shows the results of the analysis. Computed across all subjects, the correlation between behavioral variability (cvRT) and brain variability (PCA dimensionality and MSE) was negative and highly robust across most of the EEG channels (Figure 3a). The correlation for accuracy was a mirror image of the pattern for cvRT, showing a positive correlation with PCA and MSE estimates (Figure 3a). Mean RT, however, showed a much weaker, and statistically unreliable, correlation pattern with brain variability measures (Figure 3a). Finally, the correlation of chronological age and brain variability was very strong and positive across most of the scalp. The impressions derived from the visual inspection of Figure 3a were confirmed by the PLS analysis (Fig 3b). Measures of behavioral consistency (cvRT and accuracy) and chronological age showed stable correlations with the brain variability measured with PCA or MSE. Mean RT, however, did not. In other words, increased brain variability during maturation was associated with more stable and accurate behavior. 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000106.g003 Figure 3 Within-subject brain variability in relation to behavior and chronological age. A) Correlations are arranged in a table where rows represent three within-subject brain variability measures (pre- and post- stimulus PCA and MSE) and columns represent behavioral measures (cvRT, accuracy and meanRT) and age. Each entry in the table shows a scalp map resulting from interpolated values from single channel correlations between given brain variability measure and given behavior or age. Unstable correlations (where the 95% CI included 0) were set to 0. Colormap corresponds to [−0.7 0.7] range of correlation values. Along with each scalp map of correlations, there is an inset showing a scatter plot representing subject measures for a single channel (electrode O2). Subjects are grouped by color according to age group membership. Estimated value of correlation ( r ) along with the associated 95% CI is given on top. B) Results of the statistical analysis of the observed correlations. For each brain measure, the partial least squares (PLS ) analysis detected one significant pattern of correlations (p = 0). The bar graph plots the global correlation of brain variability and each behavior measure or age (+/− bootstrap estimated standard error). As can be seen, for all three brain measures PLS detected similar global patterns of simultaneous negative correlation with cvRT, positive correlation with accuracy and age, and no stable correlation with mean RT. For each of the three within-subject brain variability measures, the bootstrap analysis of PLS confirmed that the correlation patterns were robustly expressed across most channels (data not shown). The statement that the relation between behavioral variability and brain variability is mediated by maturation implies that if age differences were eliminated, the strong correlations seen in Figure 3 with cvRT and accuracy would be reduced. This turned out to be correct. We again used PLS to analyze the relation between the brain variability and behavior measures when chronological age was regressed out of both sets of measures. For cvRT and accuracy, the correlations for the full sample were reduced, and in the case of MSE, no longer statistically reliable (Figure 4). The effect was less dramatic when the analysis was performed on children only. Furthermore, the correlation between mean RT and brain variability remained nonsignificant after adjusting for chronological age for both the entire sample and the children. The age-adjustment process indicated that a large proportion of the relationship between behavioral consistency and brain variability was due to maturation, as measured by chronological age. 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000106.g004 Figure 4 Relationship between within-subject brain variability and behavior after adjusting for age differences across individuals (age was regressed out from all measures). Color scale and legends are the same as for Figure 3. A) Correlations are arranged in a table where rows represent three within-subject brain variability measures (pre- and post- stimulus PCA and MSE) and columns represent three behavioral measures (cvRT, accuracy and mean RT). Each entry in the table shows a scalp map resulting from interpolated values from single channel correlations between given brain variability measure and given behavior or age. Unstable correlations (where the 95% CI included 0) were set to 0. Colormap corresponds to [−0.7 0.7] range of correlation values. Along with each scalp map of correlations, there is an inset showing a scatter plot representing subject measures for a single channel (electrode O2). Subjects are grouped by color according to age group membership. Estimated value of correlation (r) along with the associated 95% CI is given on top. B) Statistical analysis of brain-behavior correlations adjusted for age. PLS detected one significant pattern of correlations for pre- and post-stimulus PCA dimensionality estimation (p = 0.003, and p = 0.011), and one pattern for MSE that did not exceed conventional statistical thresholds (p = 0.131). Bootstrap analysis of the two significant patterns for PCA showed, however, that the patterns were unstable for most channels (data not shown). Discussion In contrast to behavioral variability, brain variability increases with maturation. With maturation comes differentiation and specialization of brain regions, but at the same time there is increased integration between distributed neuronal populations and establishment of new functional connections [29]. The change in balance between differentiation and specialization would produce more variability in on-going activity as the number of simultaneous processes possible at any given moment increases. Mature and integrated nervous systems generally have more prolonged and complicated neural transients [30]. Such transients are characteristic of a system with high neural complexity [31]. With the maturational increase in brain signal variability there is an increase in behavioral stability. Across the sample we studied, subjects with higher signal variability showed less variability in response latency (measured with cvRT) and greater performance accuracy. When the measures were adjusted for the chronological age, the relationship between brain and behavioral variability weakened, suggesting that a large part of the relationship represented a maturational effect. It is noteworthy that mean reaction time, which also showed a maturational change, did not significantly correlate with brain variability. It may be that other physiological factors are more important for the response speed change during maturation. By contrast, strong correlations with behavioral consistency indicates that cvRT and accuracy are likely tapping into aspects of the behavioral tuning which are more tightly related to the changes in brain complexity/variability. The present results may seem at odds with the intuitive notion of behavior and brain variability, where one would expect that they go hand in hand. However, the results do make sense when the nonlinear dynamics of the nervous system are considered. Internal variability may be vital to enable the brain to parse weak and ambiguous incoming signals [10],[32],[33]. Variability can facilitate the exchange signals between neurons [34],[35], transitions in metastable systems [7], and the formation of functional networks [17],[36]. As the nervous system matures, physiological variability increases, which is captured by increases in complexity [37],[38], and the system can better adapt to its environment. Maturational changes that have been reported in children's evoked potentials can also be related to increased brain signal complexity. Compared to adults, the average evoked responses in children tends to show higher amplitude and longer latency on early responses, and less well-defined later responses [20],[21],[39]. Spectral power distribution also changes with maturation, with a gradual reduction in low frequencies and an increase in higher frequencies [22],[40]. The relative change in spectral power density presumably underlies the reduction in the latency of the evoked responses and its multicomponent nature, where the lower frequency bias in children would yield slow and broad evoked potentials. The emergence of higher frequencies with maturation would both decrease the evoked response latency and allow additional deflections to emerge (i.e., N100, P200, etc.). Both the spectral power and evoked response changes would be expected given the maturational increase in complexity. The emergence of higher frequencies would reflect the enhanced local processing (segregation), whereas the multicomponent evoked response is thought to reflect reentrant interactions [41], suggesting enhanced integration. In the age range of the children we studied the brain is in a state of structural and functional refinement [42],[43]. Myelination and neural pruning increase differentiation of information flow in the brain, enabling a shift from a system that responds in a slow and stimulus-locked manner, to one that responds more rapidly and where the internal variability reflects the parallel exploration of the functional repertoire before converging to an optimal response [16],[44]. In the case of normal development, the increased variability leads to a stabilization of behavior, increasing the cognitive repertoire of the system. One may postulate that internal variability would mature to some optimum level, based on both physiology and experience, but that further increases or decreases, coming from disease or damage would compromise behavioral stability. The suggestions derived from the present findings contribute to the growing evidence that internal dynamics are a key feature governing brain function [45]–[47]. Materials and Methods Subjects EEG recordings were collected from 24 adults and 55 children for a total of 79 subjects. Adults (18 females) ranged from 20 to 33 years of age. Children were divided into four age groups as follows: 8–9 years (n = 11, 3 females), 10–11 years (n = 16, 8 females), 12–13 years (n = 15, 8 females), and 14–15 years (n = 13, 6 females). Adult subjects and children, along with their parents, signed informed written consent. All subjects were healthy with no known cognitive or neurological disorders and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. All children successfully completed two sub-tests of the WISC III (vocabulary and block design). The experimental procedure was approved by the French Comite Operationnel pour l'Ethique dans les Sciences de la Vie du CNRS. Apparatus and task Continuous EEG was recorded (NeuroScan 4.1) on an EasyCap (10/10 system) containing 32 electrodes and Cz as reference, sampling rate of 500 Hz, a band-pass 0.1–100 Hz, and a gain of 500 (SynAmps). Subjects performed a rapid face recognition task. Each trial started with a presentation of a novel or familiar face for 500ms and subjects responded by pressing either a target or a non-target button depending on whether they recognized the face. Detailed description of the stimuli and the task are given in Itier & Taylor [19]. Data preprocessing Infraorbital electrodes for measuring eye movements were removed and an average reference was computed. The final number of electrodes was 31. Continuous EEG recordings were lowpass filtered at 40 Hz. Data were epoched and baselined into [−200 1200] ms epochs with a [−200 0] ms pre-stimulus baseline. Preliminary artifact removal was performed using independent component analysis (ICA) as implemented in EEGLAB software [48]. Trials contaminated with excessive amplitudes were removed first, then ICA decomposition was performed on the remaining concatenated trials and components carrying ocular and muscle artifacts were subtracted. The number of kept trials per subject was between 236 and 761, with an average of 529. For the signal variability estimation, it was important to have equal amounts of artifact-free data across subjects. We therefore introduced an additional trial selection step based on the total global field power (gfp), calculated as a sum of squared amplitudes across all electrodes and all time points for the trial duration. The 100 trials closest to the median for each subject were selected for further variability analysis. This selection criterion minimized potential presence of trials contaminated with high residual amplitude artifacts. This was particularly important for the convergence of multiscale entropy (MSE) algorithm (see below). It is critical to note that, with the exception of MSE, the results were similar when all trials that passed initial screening were analyzed. Behavioral measures For each subject we calculated two response time related measures: mean response time (mean RT) and coefficient of variation of the response time (cvRT). The coefficient of variation of RT was calculated as the standard deviation divided by the mean RT within subject, and was taken as a measure of subject's behavioral variability. The scaling procedure in cvRT minimizes differences between groups that arise from differences in mean and standard deviations. Mean and cvRT were based on thresholded RTs ( 14 were not calculated because the corresponding coarse-grained (downsampled) time series were too short ( 10 Hz) and decrease in relative contributions from low frequencies (>10 Hz). B) PLS detected one significant pattern (p = 0) of linear changes related to maturation. C) Bootstrap analysis of pattern expressions across channels and frequencies. For each frequency and each channel, bootstrap ratios were thresholded (pattern was stably expressed if absolute bootstrap ratio exceeded 3.5). From the thresholded values for each frequency, a scalp map was interpolated across channels. The resulting bootstrap ratio maps indicate regions of stable expressions of the pattern from B), with red and blue regions corresponding to stable positive and negative expressions, respectively. Grey regions indicate regions of no stable expression. A stable pattern of increase in spectral contribution was observed at higher frequencies (> = 15 Hz). Inverted pattern, i.e., decrease in spectral contribution was observed at 5 Hz only. At 10 Hz, there was no stable pattern expression. (0.49 MB EPS) Click here for additional data file. Figure S2 Modifying signal by biasing spectral power distribution towards lower frequencies. Relative increase in lower frequencies is paralleled by relative decrease in higher frequencies, while the total spectral power remains unchanged. This is shown in panel B. Panel A shows single trial time series of original and modified signals. Note the effect of smoothing due to the decrease in higher frequencies. Comparative results for PCA and MSE are shown in panels C and D, respectively. Error bars in the MSE graphs represent standard errors associated with mean MSE across single trials. (0.57 MB EPS) Click here for additional data file. Figure S3 Modifying signal by randomizing phase while keeping power spectrum unchanged. Panel Ashows single trial time series of original and modified signals. Panel B shows average signal across trials. Note how typical ERP components like P1, N1 and P2 are missing in the modified signal because randomization procedure destroyed any phase relationship across trials. Comparative results for PCA and MSE are shown in panels C & D, respectively. Error bars in the MSE graphs represent standard errors associated with mean MSE across single trials. (0.67 MB EPS) Click here for additional data file.
                Bookmark

                Author and article information

                Contributors
                Ole.Fasmer@uib.no
                espenborga.johansen@hioa.no
                Journal
                Behav Brain Funct
                Behav Brain Funct
                Behavioral and Brain Functions : BBF
                BioMed Central (London )
                1744-9081
                1 December 2016
                1 December 2016
                2016
                : 12
                : 32
                Affiliations
                [1 ]Department of Clinical Medicine, Section for Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine and Dentistry, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway
                [2 ]Division of Psychiatry, Haukeland University Hospital, Bergen, Norway
                [3 ]K.G. Jebsen Centre for Research on Neuropsychiatric Disorders, Bergen, Norway
                [4 ]Oslo and Akershus University College, Stensberggata 26, 0170 Oslo, Norway
                Article
                117
                10.1186/s12993-016-0117-9
                5131462
                27906019
                f89d5b13-2997-49dc-9638-ca932094e36b
                © The Author(s) 2016

                Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License ( http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver ( http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

                History
                : 11 June 2016
                : 22 November 2016
                Funding
                Funded by: Norges Forskningsråd (NO)
                Award ID: NFR 175096
                Award ID: 173046
                Funded by: European Commission (BE)
                Award ID: Food-CT-2006-022923-ATHON
                Categories
                Research
                Custom metadata
                © The Author(s) 2016

                Neurology
                adhd,shr,wky,behavioral variability,motor activity,video-analyses
                Neurology
                adhd, shr, wky, behavioral variability, motor activity, video-analyses

                Comments

                Comment on this article