6
views
0
recommends
+1 Recommend
0 collections
    0
    shares
      • Record: found
      • Abstract: found
      • Article: found
      Is Open Access

      Editorial: Manual Skills, Handedness, and the Organization of Language in the Brain

      editorial

      Read this article at

      Bookmark
          There is no author summary for this article yet. Authors can add summaries to their articles on ScienceOpen to make them more accessible to a non-specialist audience.

          Abstract

          Hand preference and cerebral dominance for some aspects of language processing are hallmarks of human brain functioning. Yet, their mutual relationships, similar to interrelations between hemispheric dominance for low-level sensorimotor control of the hand and the representations of higher-order, skilled actions (praxis) still remain unclear. Whereas in some accounts (Liepmann, 1900, 1908; Geschwind and Galaburda, 1985; Heilman, 1997; see also Goldenberg, 2013b) right handedness reflects (at least in part) the functioning of the left-lateralized manual praxis system, evidence from majority of left-handers weakens such a notion because they often represent praxis skills in their motor non-dominant—left—hemispheres, too (Lausberg et al., 1999; Frey et al., 2005; Goldenberg, 2013a; see also Gonzalez and Goodale, 2009; Grabowska et al., 2012; Haberling and Corballis, 2015; Kroliczak et al., 2016; cf. Carey et al., 2015). Although the putative links between praxis and language, and their interactions with handedness, have been long considered (Dejerine and Andre-Thomas, 1912; Heilman et al., 1973, 1974; McManus, 1985; Annett and Alexander, 1996; Meador et al., 1999), more recent studies clarify their relationships (Króliczak et al., 2011; Vingerhoets et al., 2013; Biduła and Króliczak, 2015; Goldenberg and Randerath, 2015; cf. Goldenberg, 2013b), further strengthening the idea that they are contingent on each other (Vingerhoets, 2014; Króliczak et al., 2018). Moreover, evidence from individuals with rarer forms of brain dominance now supports the idea that there is a longstanding evolutionary origin to the cerebral arrangement and distribution of both related and complementary skills, e.g., praxis and language vs. attention (Grabowska et al., 1994; Corballis, 2003; Cai et al., 2013; Goldenberg, 2013b). The primary goal of this Research Topic is to present new pieces of evidence on the neural and functional organization of language and praxis, their links (or lack of thereof) with handedness and low-level motor skills, as well as behavioral consequences of their representations for other functions. Among the 12 contributing Original Research Articles, the considered functions include short-term tactile learning of Braille reading, visual word and number processing, and visuospatial discrimination. Yet, because the neural underpinnings of these functions are often strongly lateralized in the human brain, and may have common ancestry, their evolution and development is discussed in two Hypothesis and Theory articles. Evolution and Early Development of Cerebral and Behavioral Asymmetries The evolution of language and tool manufacture is considered by Corballis in the context of behavioral asymmetries that emerged in humans. Evidence is discussed that such asymmetries must have developed in an independent manner, triggered by multi-genetic sources, rather than a single overriding principle. It is also emphasized that representations of language and tool-use skills are rather poorly correlated with handedness. The relation of the individual development of hand preference to the critical development of human basic sensorimotor and cognitive abilities is, nevertheless, assumed in Michel. Based on earlier ideas that hand preference acquisition precedes unimanual object manipulation, and that both these skills must precede role-differentiated bimanual manipulation of objects, this contribution provides a description of an ideal paradigm for testing their development and relationships. The importance of studying developmental differences of cognitive skills across handedness is also emphasized. Language Laterality, its Sources, and (in)Activity-Dependent Word Processing and Learning Unique characteristics of atypical organization of language are considered by Biduła et al. Whereas, most of atypical cases are indeed found in left-handers, they are also present in ambidextrous and right-handed people (cf. Carey and Johnstone, 2014). Indeed, Biduła et al. demonstrated that although group results indicate mirror-reversed organization of language in atypical participants, evidence for this is less compelling at an individual level of analysis. The relationships between language laterality and handedness are also discussed by Schmitz et al., but now from the point of genetic influences. Evidence is shown that handedness and language organization are complex phenotypes that are ontogenetically independent. This report ends with conclusions that genes involved in ontogenesis of handedness contribute primarily to structural development, whereas genes underlying language laterality also contribute to the development of other cognitive processes (but seem also associated with mental and neurological disorders). Given language-praxis links, certain kinds of actions, or inaction, could affect language processing and learning. For example, changes in motor system functioning could flexibly influence comprehension and acquisition of words (cf. Shebani and Pülvermuller, 2018). An intriguing paper by Yasuda et al. demonstrates that while peripheral body states influence action verb processing, in contrast to a strong embodiment view, constrained arm posture affected responses to both manual and non-manual action verbs. The opposite issue, that is, an impact of word processing on movement kinematics was investigated by Rugani et al. They showed that automatic numerical processing affects action execution in a context of kicking small balls with the index finger. Their participants responded faster to small numbers while kicking the ball to the left, and vice versa. Notably, Rugani et al. argue that similar paradigms could be used to study the impact of cognition on action in an unbiased way. Learning new vocabulary can be a challenge, especially in elder people. Yet, as Heim and collaborators show (Heim et al.), a nap, in contrast to activity or even rest, helps to consolidate language learning. While these results are less relevant to the language-praxis debate, their translation to clinical settings for improvement of speech-language therapy following brain injuries would be welcome. Still, in some circumstances learning to read new words is not possible without the involvement of certain kinds of actions, as in tactile learning of Braille (Debowska et al.). This study established that even short-term tactile training can introduce functional and structural changes in the fusiform gyrus, linked to visual processing of language, including single word reading. This is yet another demonstration how language and praxis can be related. When Handedness Does Not Matter, Does it? Some manual actions seem so simple that one would expect mainly contralateral control of their performance. Yet, as Begliomini et al. show, grasping with the left (dominant) hand in left handers is not controlled only by the right (contralateral) hemisphere. They found increased connectivity with the left hemisphere parieto-frontal resources. Notably, the right (non-dominant) hand is controlled as in right-handers. These outcomes are consistent with a notion that hemispheric specialization for higher-order visuomotor control does not depend on handedness (Gonzalez et al., 2006). Nevertheless, reports on the impact of handedness, the used hand/eye, and/or other cognitive abilities on performance of the line bisection task (Ochando and Zago), and left-right discrimination (Constant and Mellet) reveal a more complex picture. In the line bisection task, performance depends on integration of differently weighted visuospatial hemispheric mechanisms, the motor component of the used hand, and individual laterality factors. When they are congruent, the strongest behavioral biases are observed. As to left-right discrimination, left-handers were found better at identifying their left hands and verifying “left” propositions. Nonetheless, numerous interactions of other factors provide new insights into the links between cognitive skills and left-right discrimination. Functions Still To-be-Tested in Left-Handers The last three papers focus entirely on specific aspects of motor control. They shed new light on the impact of spatial alignment and response hand in processing visual illusions (Scocchia et al.), competition between functional and situational affordances (Roche and Chainay), and the influence of action mode on efficiency in rule- vs. plan-based movements (Scheib et al.). Of course, the studied skills are less likely to depend on linguistic representations. Yet, although some differences contingent on the responding hand were suggested, they are less likely to emerge when directed at tools. As such, these approaches can stimulate new research and reveal new findings of theoretical interest for our debate. Conclusions This Research Topic highlights the findings on the relationships between manual skills and language, and their putative links to handedness and associated motor functions. Research showing both similarities and disparities in their organization in right-handed and left-handed (but also ambidextrous) individuals is featured. The debate includes the evolution and early development of cerebral and behavioral asymmetries, as well as their genetic foundations. We hope that further discussions and research ideas will emerge out of this work. Author Contributions GK conceived this work and drafted the editorial. CG and DC contributed intellectually to this work, revised, and approved the draft for publication. Conflict of Interest Statement The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

          Related collections

          Most cited references29

          • Record: found
          • Abstract: not found
          • Article: not found

          Cerebral lateralization. Biological mechanisms, associations, and pathology: I. A hypothesis and a program for research.

            Bookmark
            • Record: found
            • Abstract: found
            • Article: not found

            Complementary hemispheric specialization for language production and visuospatial attention.

            Language production and spatial attention are the most salient lateralized cerebral functions, and their complementary specialization has been observed in the majority of the population. To investigate whether the complementary specialization has a causal origin (the lateralization of one function causes the opposite lateralization of the other) or rather is a statistical phenomenon (different functions lateralize independently), we determined the lateralization for spatial attention in a group of individuals with known atypical right hemispheric (RH) lateralization for speech production, based on a previous large-scale screening of left-handers. We show that all 13 participants with RH language dominance have left-hemispheric dominance for spatial attention, and all but one of 16 participants with left-hemispheric language dominance are RH dominant for spatial attention. Activity was observed in the dorsal fronto-parietal pathway of attention, including the inferior parietal sulcus and superior parietal lobule, the frontal eye-movement field, and the inferior frontal sulcus/gyrus, and these regions functionally colateralized in the hemisphere dominant for attention, independently of the side of lateralization. Our results clearly support the Causal hypothesis about the complementary specialization, and we speculate that it derives from a longstanding evolutionary origin. We also suggest that the conclusions about lateralization based on an unselected sample of the population and laterality assessment using coarse functional transcranial Doppler sonography should be interpreted with more caution.
              Bookmark
              • Record: found
              • Abstract: found
              • Article: not found

              From mouth to hand: gesture, speech, and the evolution of right-handedness.

              The strong predominance of right-handedness appears to be a uniquely human characteristic, whereas the left-cerebral dominance for vocalization occurs in many species, including frogs, birds, and mammals. Right-handedness may have arisen because of an association between manual gestures and vocalization in the evolution of language. I argue that language evolved from manual gestures, gradually incorporating vocal elements. The transition may be traced through changes in the function of Broca's area. Its homologue in monkeys has nothing to do with vocal control, but contains the so-called "mirror neurons," the code for both the production of manual reaching movements and the perception of the same movements performed by others. This system is bilateral in monkeys, but predominantly left-hemispheric in humans, and in humans is involved with vocalization as well as manual actions. There is evidence that Broca's area is enlarged on the left side in Homo habilis, suggesting that a link between gesture and vocalization may go back at least two million years, although other evidence suggests that speech may not have become fully autonomous until Homo sapiens appeared some 170,000 years ago, or perhaps even later. The removal of manual gesture as a necessary component of language may explain the rapid advance of technology, allowing late migrations of Homo sapiens from Africa to replace all other hominids in other parts of the world, including the Neanderthals in Europe and Homo erectus in Asia. Nevertheless, the long association of vocalization with manual gesture left us a legacy of right-handedness.
                Bookmark

                Author and article information

                Contributors
                Journal
                Front Psychol
                Front Psychol
                Front. Psychol.
                Frontiers in Psychology
                Frontiers Media S.A.
                1664-1078
                26 April 2019
                2019
                : 10
                : 930
                Affiliations
                [1] 1Action and Cognition Laboratory, Department of Social Sciences, Institute of Psychology, Adam Mickiewicz University in Poznan , Poznan, Poland
                [2] 2Department of Kinesiology, University of Lethbridge , Lethbridge, AB, Canada
                [3] 3Department of Neuroscience, University of Lethbridge , Lethbridge, AB, Canada
                [4] 4Perception, Action and Memory Research Group, School of Psychology, Bangor University , Bangor, United Kingdom
                Author notes

                Edited and reviewed by: Britt Anderson, University of Waterloo, Canada

                *Correspondence: Gregory Króliczak krolgreg@ 123456amu.edu.pl

                This article was submitted to Perception Science, a section of the journal Frontiers in Psychology

                Article
                10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00930
                6497748
                31080429
                41b7146f-1f9c-4442-a1f4-2281177ac2b1
                Copyright © 2019 Króliczak, Gonzalez and Carey.

                This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

                History
                : 15 March 2019
                : 08 April 2019
                Page count
                Figures: 0, Tables: 0, Equations: 0, References: 31, Pages: 4, Words: 2483
                Categories
                Psychology
                Editorial

                Clinical Psychology & Psychiatry
                hand preference,cerebral dominance,brain functioning,sensorimotor control,higher-order processing,skilled actions,praxis

                Comments

                Comment on this article