686
views
0
recommends
+1 Recommend
1 collections
    1
    shares
      scite_
       
      • Record: found
      • Abstract: found
      • Article: found
      Is Open Access

      Meta-analysis: on average, undergraduate students’ intelligence is merely average

      Published
      research-article
      Bookmark

            Abstract

            Background:

            According to a widespread belief, the average IQ of university students is 115 to 130 IQ points, that is, substantially higher than the average IQ of the general population (M = 100, SD = 15). We traced the origin of this belief to obsolete intelligence data collected in 1940s and 1950s when university education was the privilege of a few. Examination of more recent IQ data indicate that IQ of university students and university graduates dropped to the average of the general population. The decline in students’ IQ is a necessary consequence of increasing educational attainment over the last 80 years. Today, graduating from university is more common than completing high school in the 1940s.

            Method:

            We conducted a meta-analysis of the mean IQ scores of college and university students samples tested with Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale between 1939 and 2022.

            Results:

            The results show that the average IQ of undergraduate students today is a mere 102 IQ points and declined by approximately 0.2 IQ points per year. The students’ IQ also varies substantially across universities and is correlated with the selectivity of universities (measured by average SAT scores of admitted students).

            Discussion:

            These findings have wide-ranging implications. First, universities and professors need to realize that students are no longer extraordinary but merely average, and have to adjust curricula and academic standards. Second, employers can no longer rely on applicants with university degrees to be more capable or smarter than those without degrees. Third, students need to realize that acceptance into university is no longer an invitation to join an elite group. Fourth, the myth of brilliant undergraduate students in scientific and popular literature needs to be dispelled. Fifth, estimating premorbid IQ based on educational attainment is vastly inaccurate, obsolete, not evidence based, and mere wishful thinking. Sixth, obsolete IQ data or tests ought not to be used to make high-stakes decisions about individuals, for example, by clinical psychologists to opine about the intelligence and cognitive abilities of their clients.

            Main article text

            INTRODUCTION

            What is the average IQ of undergraduate students? According to a widespread belief, the average IQ of university students is somewhere between 115 to 130, that is, substantially higher than the average IQ of the general population (M = 100, SD =15). For example, in a series of widely cited articles on intelligence, life chances, and occupational success, Gottfredson [14] maintained that undergraduate students’ IQ (labeled “College Format” IQ in her papers) ranged from 112 to 120. Figure 1 is an adaptation of the figures published in several of Gottfredson’s articles. The figure shows the bell curve symmetrical distribution of IQ scores, with a mean of 100 and a standard deviation of 15, with “life chances”, “training potential”, and “career potential” marked within the figure. Similarly, in Assessing Adolescent and Adult Intelligence, Kaufman and Lichtenberger [7] wrote that college graduates’ average IQ is 115 (see p. 16, Figure 1.1), citing as sources of this information Matarazzo ([8], p. 178); Jensen ([9], p. 113); and Reynolds et al. [10]. Kaufman and Lichtenberger [7] also cite Heaton et al. [11], unpublished manuscript, to claim that college graduates’ mean IQ on the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale III (WAIS-III) standardization sample was 116.8. (p. 115). More recently, in the classic text Neuropsychological Assessment, Lezak et al. [12] wrote that “the average college graduate typically scores one to two standard deviations [115 to 130 IQ points] above the general population mean on tests of this type [vocabulary tests]” (p. 167), citing Anastasi [13] as the source of this information. Not surprisingly, the notion that undergraduate students’ IQ is substantially higher than that of general population found its way into popular magazines. For example, Scientific American published an article by Gottfredson [2] with a version of Figure 1 included and the “college format” having an IQ in the range of 112 to 120. More recently, Henderson [14], wrote, in Psychology Today, that “the average IQ of a college graduate is about 114.”

            Figure 1.

            WAIS [5] FSIQ, career potential, training potential and life chances as per Gottfredson [14]. Gottfredson’s views are based on Wonderlic Personnel Test (WPT) [6] data translated to WAIS FSIQ [5] and published in Wonderlic [6].

            In this article, we first examine the origins of this belief of brilliant undergraduate students. Second, we critically review the existing evidence demonstrating that this belief is a myth - a fairy tale from a bygone era that only a few still living remember. Third, we report a new study that examined changes in undergraduate students IQs from 1940s to present. Fourth, we discuss wide-ranging implications of our findings as well as the disastrous consequences of believing the fairy tales of very smart undergraduate students.

            The origins of the belief of brilliant undergraduate students

            What is the origin of this belief of brilliant undergraduate students? Careful examination of data cited in support of this belief shows that the data is (a) obsolete, collected decades or nearly a century ago, (b) often not representative of general nor specific populations, (c) often collected under unknown conditions and circumstances, and (d) often so poorly described that the very basic characteristics of samples cannot be established. For example, Gottfredson [1] cited data from the Wonderlic Personnel Test (WPT) [6] – a 20 minute, 50 item long multiple choice test – to support her strong claims about the relationship between IQ and life chances, training style, career potential, as well as her claim that IQ of undergraduate students ranges from 112 to 120. Wonderlic [6] itself states that the “mean score for college freshmen” is WAIS IQ 115 or WPT 24 and that “college graduate mean [WAIS] IQ [is] 120” or WPT 29 (see p. 26). However, within Wonderlic’s [6] sample, college graduates’ IQ actually ranged from 80 to over 146 WAIS IQ points (see Wonderlic [6], p. 25, for a range of WPT scores and p. 20 for translation of WPT scores to WAIS Full Scale IQ (FSIQ). Most critically, Wonderlic’s [6] “norms” (p. 25) and specific occupation norms (p. 27) are actually not norms at all; they are scores of some job applicants somewhere, assessed under unknown circumstances, and assessed by unknown assessors. Examinees were never sampled to match any population census data, were not tested under standardized conditions, and nearly nothing is known about the examinees themselves. In fact, Wonderlic [6] indicates that the scores were reported back to Wonderlic Personnel Test Inc. by various companies that decided to use WPT to examine job applicants. For example, “Teacher” norms with a mean WPT of 26 or WAIS FSIQ of 113 were reported back by ten unknown companies and reflected scores of 500 applicants for some unspecified teaching jobs (see p. 27). No other information was provided about these teaching job applicants, including their age, education level, or primary teaching assignments (e.g., early childhood, elementary, secondary/high school, college).

            Similarly, Matarazzo [8], Kaufman and Lichtenberger’s [7] first source, states that the WAIS IQ of college graduates is 115 (see Table 7.3 in Matarazzo [8]) and informs that the data in the table “is based on our own clinical experience and should provide the interested reader with data for a good working rule of thumb [emphasis added]” (p. 178). Jensen [9], Kaufman and Lichtenberger’s [7] second source, states that the mean IQ of college graduates is 120 and the mean IQ of “freshmen in typical four-year college” is 115 and states that these estimates were “compiled by Cronbach ([15], p. 174)”. In turn, Cronbach [15] cites several sources published between 1930 and 1958, including a review of previously published studies by Plant and Richardson [16] who concluded that an average college students’ Wechsler-Bellevue Intelligence Scale (WBIS) [17] FSIQ is 120, and the average college freshmen WBIS FSIQ is 116 (p. 230). Reynold et al. [10], Kaufman and Lichtenberger’s [7] third source, gives the mean WAIS-R FSIQ of college graduates (i.e., individuals with 16 or more years of education, including those with MA and PhD degrees) as 115.17 based on 244 adults of all ages with at least that level of education in WAIS-R [18] normative sample (tested in 1980). Heaton et al. [11], Kaufman and Lichtemberger’s [7] source for WAIS-III FSIQ of college graduates being 116.8, could not be examined as it was not published. However, Longman et al. [19] analysis of the same WAIS-III normative sample showed that college graduates, that is, those with 16 or more years of education, had the mean WAIS-III FSIQ of only 111.6 (p. 429). Finally, Lezak et al.’s [12] only citation is Anastasi [13], also an ancient text.

            Major reasons why undergraduate students’ IQ cannot be as high as 115 or even higher

            There are three major reasons why undergraduate students’ average IQ today cannot be 115 or even higher as estimated by obsolete data collected 70 or 80 years ago: generational increases in intelligence called Flynn Effect, massive increases in educational attainment, and structure of WAIS normative data.

            Flynn Effect

            IQ scores have been rising at a rate of 0.3 per year or 3 IQ points per decade [2022]. As a result, an examinee scoring 115 on an intelligence test normed in 1950 would score only 93 on an intelligence test normed in 2022. Flynn Effect is observed in successive versions of perhaps one of the most commonly used intelligence tests – WAIS and its predecessor WBIS. The WBIS sample was “mostly urban from the City and State of New York” and exclusively Caucasian, and thus, not representative of the US population [17], whereas WAIS versions samples were designed to be representative of the US population [5,18,23,24].

            Table 1 shows the mean Verbal IQ (VIQ), Verbal Comprehension Index (VCI), Performance IQ (PIQ), Perceptual Reasoning Index (PRI), and FSIQ scores of three samples of examinees, each completing two temporally adjacent versions of WAIS, the IQ differences between the two adjacent WAIS versions, and the overall cumulative difference between the WAIS and WAIS-IV mean IQ. Over 53 years between WAIS-IV and WAIS, FSIQ increased by 13.3 points or 0.25 per year. Thus, if an average teacher’s WAIS FSIQ was truly 113, as Wonderlic [6] claimed, this same average teacher would be expected to score only 99.7 points when assessed by the more recently normed and up-to-date WAIS-IV. Using 0.3 IQ points per year – an estimate based on a much larger set of studies – this same average teacher would be expected to score only 97.1.

            Table 1:

            VIQ/VCI, PIQ/PRI, and FSIQ scores of three samples, each tested with two successive versions of Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scales (US Editions).

            WAIS-IVWAIS-IIIΔWAIS-IIIWAIS-RΔWAIS-RWAISΔCumulative Δ
            VIQ/VCI100.1102.8−2.7102.2103.4−1.2101.8108.7−6.9−10.8
            PIQ/PRI100.3102.5−2.2103.5108.3−4.8105.4113.4−8.0−15.0
            FSIQ100.0102.9−2.9102.9105.8−2.9103.8111.3−7.5−13.3

            Note. Δ = the difference between the two means; WAIS-IV/WAIS-III sample: N = 240, aged 16–88 years ([24], p. 75); WAIS-III/WAIS-R sample: N = 192, aged 16–74 ([23], p. 79); WAIS-R/WAIS: N = 72, aged 35–44 ([18], p. 47).

            Simply put, the Flynn Effect makes it clear that it is unwarranted and patently wrong to use decades-old IQ data to make claims about the IQ of populations, samples, or individuals today. It is also unwarranted and patently wrong to compare the IQ scores obtained by samples or individuals on today’s intelligence tests to outdated IQ data on tests normed decades or nearly a century ago.

            Fletcher et al. [20] put this succinctly:

            We would not expect pediatricians to use a height/weight chart from another country or century to assess a child’s percentile rank in height or weight; if they did, we would expect corrections so that the percentile reflects the current, national distribution. Correcting an IQ score is a simple procedure that avoids having to change standards. Thus, if 15-year-old IQ norms are used, either the score itself must be corrected by about 4.5 points (0.3 × 15 years = 4.5) or the cut-point for ID [intellectual disability] needs to be corrected to 74.5 because the mean IQ of a contemporary sample using the old norms would be 104.5.

            As Fletcher pointed out, if one wants to use obsolete norms for any reason, at the very least, one must adjust either the score or the norms for Flynn Effect. Trahan et al. [22] concurs that “the need to correct IQ test scores for norms obsolescence in high-stakes decision making is abundantly clear” and “especially important when IQ test scores are compared across a broad period of time…” (p. 1352). Unfortunately, these necessary adjustments to the college students’ IQ “norms” were not reported nor considered in Gottfredson [1,2,4] or Kaufman and Lichtenberger [7].

            Furthermore, it has been argued that a failure to adjust obsolete test scores or norms for Flynn Effect is unscientific, unethical, and malpractice [20,2527]. For example, Gresham and Reschly [26] observed that “failure to account for the Flynn Effect in test score interpretation in Atkins or any other cases is a violation” of Principle 9.08 Obsolete Tests and Outdated Test Results of the Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct stating, in part: “(B) Psychologists do not base such decisions or recommendations on tests and measures that are obsolete and not useful for the current purpose.”

            Similarly, Reynolds et al. [27] concluded (p. 480):

            …the failure to apply the Flynn correction [in Atkins cases] as we have described it is tantamount to malpractice. No one’s life should depend on when an IQ test was normed.

            Increases in Educational Attainment

            The proportion of the population enrolling in and graduating with university degrees has been increasing steeply since at least 1940 [28]. Figure 2 shows the proportion of the US population, aged 25 years and older, who completed high school, 1 to 3 years of college, and attained four or more years of college (i.e., the college graduates), from 1940 to 2021. Percentages of individuals with high school increased from 24.1 to 91.1, with 1 to 3 years of college from 10.0 to 63.2, and with four or more years of college from 4.6 to 37.9.

            Figure 2.

            Increases in educational attainment in USA for adults 25 years or older, from 1940 to 2021 [28].

            Basic laws of mathematics dictate that college students’ and college graduates’ IQs must have declined substantially over the last 80 years. For example, if 80% of the population pursues undergraduate education and if they were to have an average IQ of 115, the remaining 20% of the population would have to have an average IQ of only 40 to maintain the average IQ of the entire population at 100. Obviously, this is impossible given the normal distribution of IQ scores, and accordingly, the average IQ has to decline as greater and greater proportion of population pursues undergraduate education. In fact, the IQ of college students did decline substantially. Table 2 shows FSIQ by years of education for normative samples of WAIS-R (normed between 1976 and 1980 or in 1978 on average), WAIS-III (normed in 1996), and WAIS-IV (normed from March 2007 to April 2008 or, taking a midpoint, in 2007). Over 29 years, the FSIQ of college graduates (i.e., 16 or more years of education) dropped from 115.3 to 107.4, or 0.27 IQ points per year. Similarly, the IQ of examinees with some college education (1 to 3 years) who did not (yet) graduate dropped from 107.4 to 101.4. Finally, the IQ of examinees who attended at least some college (i.e., 13 years of education or more) dropped to FSIQ 104.5 by the 2008 standardization of WAIS-IV. WAIS normative sample data confirm that college students’ and college graduates’ IQs have dropped far below the levels they once were and suggests that college students’ and graduates’ IQs today are not appreciably different from the average IQ of the entire population.

            Table 2:

            Mean FSIQ (with SDs in parentheses) by years of education for WAIS-R, WAIS-III, and WAIS-IV US Edition normative samples and WAIS-III CDN Edition normative samples.

            WAISYear0–788 or less9–111213–151617–18>1816+
            US Edition
             WAIS-R198182.2 (13.6) n = 13390.7 (12.0) n = 15896.4 (14.3) n = 472100.1 (12.6) n = 652107.4 (11.1) n = 251115.3 (12.2) n = 214
             WAIS-III199785.8 (15.1) n = 28491.2 (12.6) n = 28999.2 (12.8) n = 853103.6 (12.3) n = 579111.6 (13.2) n = 445
             WAIS-IV200882 (12.6) n = 22086.4 (13.8) n = 24396.2 (13.7) n = 647101.4 (13.1) n = 553107.1 (14.0) n = 267107.1 (14.0) n = 297111.7 (12.5) n = 43107.4 (13.9) n = 607
            CND Edition
             WAIS-III199797.3 (13.9) n = 9098.6 (15.2) n = 204100.2 (15.5) n = 177103.8 (13.7) n = 387108.7 (14.3) n = 242

            Note. WAIS-R: Table 6 [29]; WAIS-III (US): Table 4 to 8 and WAIS-III (CDN) Table 9 to 13 [19]; WAIS-IV (US): Table 4.3 [30].

            Figure 3 shows the IQ ranges for the college graduates (i.e., individuals with 16+ years of education) and the individuals with some college education (i.e., 13–15 years of education within WAIS-R, WAIS-III, and WAIS-IV normative samples). For WAIS-IV, the most recent version of the Wechsler test, the normative sample data indicate that the IQ of the middle 95% of the college graduates (i.e., individuals with 16+ years of education) ranges from 80 to 135 (M = 107.4, SD = 13.9), and that IQ of the middle 95% of the individuals with some college education (i.e., 13–15 years of education) ranges from 76 to 127 (M = 101.4, SD = 13.1). Clearly, according to WAIS-IV normative sample data, the college graduates and individuals with some college education today (or more precisely in 2007) are, on average, merely average. Only minority of students are scoring above 110 IQ points, and are in Gottfredson’s “Out Ahead” or undergraduate students’ IQ category. Equally clearly, undergraduate students’ and graduates’ IQ today cannot be what it used to be 70 to 100 years ago.

            Figure 3.

            IQ range of the middle 95% of the college graduates (16+ years of education) and individuals with some college education (13–15 years of education), respectively, within WAIS-R, WAIS-III, and WAIS-IV US Editions normative samples.

            Structure of WAIS Normative Data Analyses

            The WAIS normative data overestimate the average IQ of today’s college students and graduates because many of the examinees included in normative samples attended colleges and/or graduated from colleges decades ago (i.e., when colleges and universities were far more selective and when average IQs of college students were much higher). Accordingly, we would expect that the average WAIS-IV FSIQ of undergraduate students (students with 13 or more years of education) as well as fresh college graduates (students with 16 or more years of education) is still lower than 104.5 and 107.4, respectively, and is close to 100.

            The undergraduate students IQ differ across universities and fields

            College admission test scores, closely related to IQ scores, show that undergraduate students’ average intelligence varies hugely with at least two other factors. First, undergraduate students’ average intelligence varies hugely with the field of study. Figure 4 shows College Board average SAT ERW (Evidence-Based Reading and Writing) and Math scores for the 2021 high school graduates who took the SAT during high school by intended college major [31]. The overall ERW and Math means of SAT users were 533 (SD = 108) and 528 (SD = 120), respectively (the two means are indicated by dotted lines). The figure shows that fields such as “Education” and “Public Administration and Social Services” are below the mean on both ERW and Math. In contrast, fields such as “Mathematics and Statistics” and “Physical Sciences” are approximately 1 SD (equivalent to about 15 IQ points) above the mean on both ERW and Math. Notably, College Board also provided SAT scores for Nationally Representative Sample [32]. The Nationally Representative Sample, that is, the sample of all high school students rather than only those who typically take the SAT, averaged 507 on ERW and 506 on Math (the two means are indicated by dashed lines), and 1010 on SAT Total. Using the Nationally Representative Sample, the difference between, for example, Education vs. Mathematic and Statistics, using the IQ scale, is over 16 IQ points (Education SAT Total 101.6 vs. Mathematic and Statistics SAT Total 117.9).

            Figure 4.

            Mean SAT ERW and Math scores for the 2021 high school graduates who took SAT during high school by intended college major SAT.

            Similarly, Figure 5 shows Educational Testing Service (ETS) average Graduate Record Exam (GRE) Verbal and Quantitative scores by the intended broad graduate major field for individuals tested between July 1, 2017 and June 2020 [33]. The overall GRE Verbal mean was 150.37 (SD = 8.59) and GRE Quantitative was 153.66 (SD = 9.44) based on over 1.5 million test takers (the two means are indicated by dotted lines). GRE data confirm large differences between the fields. For example, Education/Early Childhood means are approximately 1 SD or more below Physics and Astronomy on both GRE Quantitative and GRE Verbal. Large differences exist even within fields. For example, Education/Early Childhood means are approximately 0.5 and 1 SD below Education/Secondary on GRE Quantitative and GRE Verbal, respectively.

            Figure 5.

            Mean GRE Verbal and Quantitative scores by intended broad graduate major field for individuals tested between 2017 and 2020.

            Second, undergraduate students’ IQs also vary hugely depending on which university students are or were attending. Currently, there are over 6,000 2+ and 4 years colleges and universities in US. Some colleges and universities have open admission policies, in essence admitting anyone who graduated from high school and applied. Other colleges and universities are very selective and take only a few top percent of those who dare to apply. Importantly, approximately 2,000 US colleges and universities are included in the Integrated Postsecondary Education Data System (IPEDS). The IPEDS data are available from US National Center for Education Statistics (https://nces.ed.gov/ipeds) and include 25th and 75th percentile scores for SAT and ACT of admitted students, the number of students who applied, and the number of admitted students, allowing determination of each institutions’ admission rate. Because the data file does not include the mean nor median SAT or ACT scores, the mean was estimated by taking the midpoint between the 25th and 75th percentiles. Figure 6 shows the IPEDS data from the 2020–21 admission data file. Figure 6 top left panel shows the relationship between the means SAT Math and SAT ERW scores of admitted students, r(1082) = .95, p < .001. Figure 6 top right panel shows the relationship between the means of SAT Total and ACT Composite scores of admitted students, r(1059) = .96, p < .001. Figure 6 bottom left panel shows the relationship between admission rate and SAT Total of admitted students, r(1082) = −.51, p < .001. California Institute of Technology students have the highest SAT Total (M = 1555) and the admission rate is only 6.7%. Figure 6 bottom right panel shows the distribution of SAT Total means of admitted students – the solid vertical line represents the mean SAT Total of the Nationally Representative Sample (i.e., the sample of test takers with a presumed mean IQ of 100), and the dashed vertical lines indicate ± 1 SD. This panel shows that undergraduate students in a large proportion of these institutions have mean IQ of less than 100.

            Figure 6.

            The IPEDS data for US colleges and universities. Top left panel shows the relationship between the means SAT Math and SAT ERW scores of admitted students. Top right panel shows the relationship between the means of SAT Total and ACT Composite scores of admitted students. Bottom left panel shows the relationship between admission rate and SAT Total of admitted students. Bottom right panel shows the distribution of SAT Total means of admitted students – the solid vertical line represents the mean SAT Total of the Nationally Representative Sample and dashed vertical lines indicate ± 1 SD.

            One may argue that SAT, ACT, and GRE do not measure intelligence but rather achievement. However, numerous studies have established that SAT, ACT, and GRE are all good measures of intelligence and are widely used as intelligence measures; they are highly intercorrelated [34], highly correlated with various intelligence tests including various Wechsler tests [3539], employ similar test items as intelligence tests [37], and depend on the same underlying cognitive processes. The SAT itself is based on the Army Alpha and Beta tests and the Binet’ intelligence tests [37]. A number of researchers proposed that measures such as SAT can be used as measures of pre-morbid IQ and developed regression equations predicting Wechsler FSIQs [36,37].

            Rationale and objectives of current study

            The above review of previously published analyses of Wechsler Intelligence Tests normative samples’ IQs indicates that the IQ of undergraduate students and university graduates today has declined to near the general population IQ of 100.

            However, this evidence has several limitations. First, Wechsler normative samples describe FSIQs of examinees with 13 to 15 years of education (1 to 3 years of college or university) and 16+ years of education (university graduates, including those with MA and PhD degrees) for all adults, including those who obtained the specified level of education decades ago when only a few adults went to study to colleges and universities. Accordingly, the mean IQ of undergraduate students at any given time is likely lower than the mean IQ of all adults with the equivalent level of educational attainment. Second, the last Wechsler test was normed in 2007, some 15 years ago. Given that the proportion of the eligible population going on to pursue college and university-level education has continued to rise, the mean IQ of undergraduate students has likely continued to decline. Third, Wechsler’s normative samples are too limited to provide any insight into how much the mean IQs of undergraduate students vary across universities. The SAT (and ACT) data indicate that the range between the least and the most selective universities exceeds three standard deviations, the equivalent of 45 IQ points (see Figure 6). Accordingly, it is likely that the mean IQ of undergraduate students varies substantially across the universities and correlates with the mean SATs of admitted students. Finally, it is largely unknown how Wechsler normative samples were recruited.

            Therefore, independent evidence of the decline of the IQ of undergraduate students is both necessary and valuable to address some of the limitations detailed above and to examine the decline in undergraduate students’ IQ using different and more robust methodology. The main objective of the present study is to conduct a meta-analysis of the mean IQ scores of college and university student samples tested with Wechsler intelligence tests (WBIS, WAIS, WAIS-R, WAIS-III, WAIS-IV) reported in the literature in order to answer the following questions: First, what is the average IQ of undergraduate students today? Second, how much did undergraduate students’ IQ decline since the 1940s (since the publication of the WBIS, the first Wechsler Intelligence test)? Third, how much does mean undergraduate students’ IQ vary across the universities? Fourth, does the mean undergraduate students’ IQ correlate with the mean SAT scores of admitted students, even if these mean SAT scores were not obtained at the same time as the mean Wechsler IQs?

            METHOD

            Inclusion and exclusion criteria

            In order for a study to be included in the meta-analysis, it had to meet a set of inclusion criteria. First, the study had to report, at minimum, one of the intelligence scales or index scores (i.e., FSIQ, VIQ, PIQ, VCI, PRI, WMI, PSI). Second, the study had to use either US or Canadian WAIS versions (i.e., WBIS, WAIS, WAIS-R, WAIS-III, WAIS-IV). Third, examinees had to be tested either in Canada or USA. Fourth, examinees had to be primarily undergraduate students (we allowed a mix of undergraduate and graduate students as long as the majority of students in a sample were undergraduate students). Fifth, samples of students had to be broadly representative of typical undergraduate students. Accordingly, the samples of students selected for specific medical conditions or learning disabilities were excluded. Finally, in the case of studies that used repeated administration of the same test, we used first administration only.

            Search for relevant studies

            Figure 7 shows the PRISMA flowchart describing the search and selection of relevant undergraduate student samples. First, the APA PsycInfo, ERIC, and MEDLINE databases were searched concurrently from the earliest available date to the end of December 31, 2022. Using the “Find all my search terms”, “apply equivalent subjects” tool, and search “All text”. The terms searched were: (a) WAIS OR “Wechsler Adult” OR (Wechsler AND Bellevue), (b) university OR college OR undergraduate*, and (c) student*. Next, the three search results were combined with AND. The search identified 1,666 potentially relevant articles, chapters, dissertations, and other reports. The full text of all these potentially relevant articles was examined and 84 data sets meeting inclusion and exclusion criteria were identified. Second, the full text of all referenced articles listed in Table 2 of Sparks and Lovett [40] was examined, and seven additional data sets meeting inclusion and exclusion criteria were identified. Third, the full text of references located in all relevant articles and book chapters, retrieved by any method, were examined, and an additional 15 data sets meeting inclusion and exclusion criteria were identified. In total, the search yielded 106 samples meeting the inclusion and exclusion criteria.

            Figure 7.

            PRISMA flowchart showing the records identified, excluded, coded, and the number of coded data sets/Wechsler mean IQs.

            Recorded variables and statistical analyses

            For each study, we coded author, year of publication, publication type (e.g., journal, dissertation, report), country, university affiliation, year(s) participants were tested, the university the participants were from, Wechsler test version, number of participants, number of males and females, mean age, and means and standard deviations for intelligence scale and index scores (FSIQ, VIQ, PIQ, VCI, PRI, WMI, PSI).

            If a study did not report FSIQ, the FSIQ was estimated from VIQ or VCI using regression imputation methods (see below). To obtain FSIQ adjusted for the Flynn Effect, 0.3 IQ points/year were substracted from reported FSIQ for each year that elapsed between the standardization year and the year of testing examinees in each sample. The standardization years used for Wechsler test versions were as follows: 1938 for WBIS [17], 1954 for WAIS [5], 1980 for WAIS-R [18], 1996 for WAIS-III [23], and 2007 for WAIS-IV [24]. If the year of testing was not reported, it was estimated by subtracting two years from the publication year. If the year of testing was reported as a range of years, the midpoint of the range was taken as the estimated year of testing.

            All statistical analyses were conducted using R statistical software [41] including the metafor package [42].

            RESULTS

            The meta-analysis included 106 samples of undergraduate students representing 9,902 students in total, with the following number of students tested in each ten year period: 1,486 in 1939–1949; 1,462 in 1950–1959; 1,938 in 1960–1969; 635 in 1970–1979, 1,848 in 1980–1989; 1,025 in 1990–1999, 1,083 in 2000–2009, and 425 in 2010–2019. There were 102 samples from the USA and four samples from Canada. The meta-analysis included 18 WBIS samples, 28 WAIS samples, 40 WAIS-R samples, 17 WAIS-III samples, and 3 WAIS-IV samples. FSIQ was reported for 100 out of 106 samples and was estimated from VIQ for 5 samples and from VCI for 1 sample by regression imputation methods. The correlation between FSIQ and VIQ means was r(63) = .974, and FSIQ for the five samples was estimated using the equation: FSIQ = 4.967 + .963 * VIQ. The correlation between FSIQ and VCI means was r(3) = .981, and the FSIQ for one sample was estimated using the equation: FSIQ = 25.185 + .772 * VCI (note that VCI was rarely reported).

            Table 3 shows descriptive information for each of the 106 undergraduate student samples. The table includes the first author, publication year, affiliation of the first author or university from which each sample was drawn, estimated year of WAIS test administration, estimated median SAT of admitted students in 2021, Wechsler test version, number of students, VIQ mean, VCI mean, FSIQ mean and standard deviation, FSIQ mean and standard deviations with imputations to replace missing values (see above), and Flynn Effect adjusted FSIQ.

            Table 3:

            Descriptive data for each of the 106 undergraduate student samples included in the meta-analysis.

            First author, yearAffiliation/UniversityYearSAT Mdn Test N VIQ M VCI M FSIQ M FSIQ SD FSIQ Imp. M FSIQ Imp. SD FSIQ Adj. M
            Aaron, 1985 [43]Indiana State U1983WAIS-R5114.41159.21159.2114.1
            Abell, 1994 [44]Loyola U of Chicago19921230WAIS-R10111011112.211112.2107.4
            Acklin, 1989 [45]The Queen’s Medical Center Honolulu1987WAIS-R125109.6109.211.3109.211.3107.2
            Advokat, 2007 [46]Louisiana State U20051195WAIS-III30108.79.2108.79.2106
            Allen, 1954 [47]U of Miami19521335WBIS491237.31237.3118.8
            Allen, 1992 [48]U of Mississippi19901120WAIS50103.3103.614.3103.614.392.8
            Anderson, 1942 [49]Wilson College19401020WBIS112118.57.2118.57.2117.9
            Axelrod, 1997 [50]Urban Commuter U1995WAIS-R65100.910.8100.910.896.4
            Bass, 1985 [51]Towson State U19831120WAIS60112.1111.67.3111.67.3102.9
            Beaujean, 2006 [52]U of Missouri20041215WAIS-III2511211210109.6
            Beers, 1994 [53]Highlands Drive Veterans Administration Medical Center1992WAIS-R22102.5104.511.4104.511.4100.9
            Beglinger, 2000 [54]U of Idaho19981105WAIS-R50111.6113.89.6113.89.6108.4
            Bell, 2001 [55]The Citadel19991120WAIS-III40116.4117.2115.49.9115.49.9114.5
            Birch, 2004 [56]College At Brockport20021080WAIS-R13113.6111.49.1111.49.1104.8
            Birch, 2016 [57]College At Brockport20141080WAIS-R16117.1117.18.3117.18.3106.9
            Bishop, 1990 [58]U of Southern Mississippi1988WAIS-R60106.816.3106.816.3104.4
            *Boer, 1988 [59]Concordia College1986WAIS-R201111311113109.2
            Buchsbaum, 1985 [60]U of California19831330WAIS38115.2114.8114.810106.1
            Burris, 1983 [61]Western Kentucky U19811080WAIS-R60110.5110.111.8110.111.8109.8
            Calvin, 1955 [62]Michigan State U19531200WBIS36122.89.2122.89.2118.2
            Cannon, 2006 [63]U of Tennessee20041221.5WAIS-III81246.81246.8121.6
            Cannon, 2009 [64]U of Tennessee20071221.5WAIS-III14117.610.2117.610.2114.3
            Carson, 2005 [65]Harward U20031520WAIS-R184129.410.9129.410.9122.5
            Carvajal, 1987 [66]Emporia State U1986WAIS-R3299.3103.510.9103.510.9101.7
            Carvajal, 1991 [67]Emporia State U1988WAIS-R31106.412.4106.412.4104
            Carvajal, 1996 [68]Emporia State U1994WAIS-R44106.310912.210912.2104.8
            Clifford, 2004 [69]Villanova U20021390WAIS-III1051001001098.2
            Clifford, 2004 [69]Villanova U20021390WAIS-III101110.77.7110.77.7108.9
            Cole, 1956 [70]Occidental College19541365WBIS4612512712710122.2
            Conry, 1965 [71]San Jose State College19631125WAIS335115.1114.88114.88112.1
            Cosden, 1997 [72]U of California19951345WAIS-R50121.38121.38116.8
            *Crawford, 1985 [73]U of Alberta1983WAIS-R38110.5110.411.7110.411.7109.5
            Davis, 2016 [74]Ball State U2014WAIS-III41110.4111.19.4111.19.4105.7
            Dennis, 1978 [75]Western Kentucky U19751080WAIS310113.2112.410.1112.410.1106.1
            Detterman, 1992 [76]Case Western Reserve U19901430WAIS-R20115.67.8115.67.8112.6
            Dodd, 2000 [77]U of North Dakota/Indiana U-Purdue University19981115WAIS-R100101.89.4101.89.496.4
            Ducheneaux, 1999 [78]Und & Oglala Lakota College19971115WAIS-III4899.799.8102.1102.110101.8
            Dymond, 1950 [79]Mount Holyoke College19481385WBIS13129.312912910126
            Estes, 1946 [80]Harvard U19441520WBIS10212812712710125.2
            Faber, 2021 [81]Roosevelt U20191015WAIS-IV25100.1105.410101.8
            Feldman, 1968 [82]Northern Illinois U1966WAIS561231216.51216.5117.4
            Fishbein, 1941 [83]Temple U1939WBIS125119.58.4119.58.4119.2
            Gajar, 1989 [84]Penn State19871185WAIS-R33117.7117.79117.79115.6
            Geiselman, 1983 [85]U of California LA1981WAIS16113.4114.210106.1
            Gerberth, 1950 [86]Washington U19481115WBIS50125.67.6125.67.6122.6
            Gooding, 2004 [87]U of Wisconsin-Madison20021360WAIS-R45120.211.1120.211.1113.6
            Gregg, 2005 [88]U of Georgia20031355WAIS-III100118.113.2118.113.2116
            Gregg, 2008 [89]U of Georgia20061355WAIS-III144113.8113.211.5113.211.5110.2
            Hanna, 1968 [90]U of Alaska19651160WAIS30120.810.5120.810.5117.5
            Harrell, 2020 [91]2018WAIS-IV276102.3102.31099
            Harwood, 1967 [92]Marshall U19651042.5WAIS28109.210910910105.7
            Hopper, 2000 [93]George Fox U19981125WAIS-III46116116.710116.1
            Ickes, 1992 [94]Kent State U19891115WAIS-R95101.2101.29.9101.29.998.5
            Kelley, 1992 [95]U of Maryland19901375WAIS40111.814.6111.814.6101
            Kramar, 1955 [96]Florida State U1953.51285WBIS196113.5114.68.7114.68.7110
            Ladd, 1950 [97]Indiana U/Indiana State Teachers College19491030WBIS190119.8121.76.3121.76.3118.4
            Lassiter, 2001 [98]The Citadel Military College19991120WAIS-III60112.7113.1111.511.2111.511.2110.6
            Lewis, 1985 [99]Illinois State U19811120WAIS-R50101.810210210101.7
            Lewis, 1985 [99]Illinois State U19801120WAIS53109.911211210104.2
            *Lott, 1952 [100]U of Alberta *1950WBIS85126.71276.61276.6123.4
            Mcgee, 1984 [101]Idaho State U1978WAIS129113.5114.310107.1
            Mefferd, 1979 [102]VA Medical Centre Houston1977WAIS100118.2118.810111.9
            Menary, 1985 [103]Michigan State U19821200WAIS105112.210.3112.210.3103.8
            Merrill, 1952 [104]U of Washington19491326.5WBIS730121.78.2121.78.2118.4
            Merrill, 1953 [105]U of Washington19511326.5WBIS248117.7120.18.6120.18.6116.2
            Mishra, 1982 [106]U of Arizona19811220WAIS-R88113.9115.616.3115.616.3115.3
            Mishra, 1982 [106]U of Arizona19811220WAIS88119.7120.214.1120.214.1112.2
            Morgan, 1997 [107]U of Georgia19951355WAIS-R30104.6108.112.2108.112.2103.6
            Morris-Friehe, 1992 [108]U of Nebraska-Lincoln19901215WAIS-R31103.5101.67.4101.67.498.6
            Mosberg, 1994 [109]U of Delaware19921240WAIS-R16109.9109.910106.3
            Nobo, 1986 [110]Washburn U19841085WAIS-R3796.811.596.811.595.6
            O’hora, 2008 [111]Florida State U20061285WAIS-III81111.5101.211316.611316.6110.0
            Olsen, 1964 [112]Washington State U19611115WAIS80511411411410111.9
            Ormrod, 1990 [113]U of Northern Colorado19881090WAIS-R41114.4115.210112.8
            Paul, 1985 [114]U of California, Berkeley19831420WAIS62122.89.3122.89.3114.1
            Pilgrim, 2000 [115]U of South Dakota19981122.5WAIS-III100109.8108.6110.911.1110.911.1110.3
            Plant, 1958 [116]San Jose State College19571125WAIS732115.6115.28.8115.28.8114.3
            Quereshi, 1985 [117]Marquette U19831250WAIS72116.5119.48119.48110.7
            Quereshi, 1985 [117]Marquette U19831250WBIS72112.4118.58.7118.58.7105.0
            Quereshi, 1985 [117 Marquette U19831250WAIS-R72113.4115.79.7115.79.7114.8
            Rakusin, 1949 [118]Pennsylvania State College19471185WBIS80122125.15.8125.15.8122.4
            Ratcliff, 2010 [119]Bryn Mawr College2008WAIS-III45112.114.2112.114.2108.5
            Rossini, 1994 [120]Roosevelt U19921015WAIS-R32101.2101.310.8101.310.897.7
            Ruble, 1980 [121]Ball State U1978.5WAIS60102.8104.67.1104.67.197.2
            Salvia, 1986 [122]Pennsylavania State U19841185WAIS-R100122.7124.69124.69123.4
            Salvia, 1988 [123]Pennsylavania State U19861185WAIS-R74122.9124.59.6124.59.6122.7
            Sartain, 1946 [124]Southern Methodist U1942.51350WBIS50115.4117.510.5117.510.5116.1
            Sedlacek, 1976 [125]Washington State U1966.51115WAIS276119.71198.21198.2115.2
            Shaw, 1965 [126]State Hospital1963WAIS100119.6119.69.1119.69.1116.8
            Sheckart, 1976 [127]Towson State College19741120WAIS36101.999.499.41093.4
            Sheldon, 1959 [128]Colorado State College19571175WAIS2010910910108.1
            Small, 1987 [129]U of Nevada19851140WAIS-R28112.2110.813.3110.813.3109.3
            Smith, 1983 [130]Rosemead School Of Psychology19811180WAIS35116.1117.78.7117.78.7109.6
            Smith, 1983 [130]Rosemead School Of Psychology19811180WAIS-R35108.2109.19.7109.19.7108.8
            Sorensen, 1968 [131]Northern Illinois U1966WAIS202119.38.8119.38.8115.7
            Steisel, 1951 [132]State U of Iowa19491210WBIS34116.88116.88113.5
            Storrs, 1952 [133]U of Florida19501375WBIS50115.8118.49.4118.49.4114.8
            *Thompson, 1999 [134]Lakehead U1997WAIS-R80101102.911.1102.911.197.8
            Titus, 2002 [135]Ball State U2000WAIS-III51105.1107.311.4107.311.4106.1
            Verney, 2005 [136]San Diego State U20031195WAIS-R75101.7102.5102.51095.6
            Wall, 1962 [137]Pennsylvania State U19601185WAIS106120.6118.98.7118.98.7117.1
            Ward, 1989 [138]Texas A&M19871270WAIS-R73114.4118.2118.210116.1
            Weyandt, 2002 [139]Central Washington U20001040WAIS-R62101.1102.510.1102.510.196.5
            Whitworth, 1986 [140]U of Texas El Paso1984WAIS75107.6109.4109.410100.4
            Whitworth, 1986 [140]U of Texas El Paso1984WAIS-R75101.1103.6103.610102.4
            Young, 2020 [141]U of Texas20181340WAIS-IV6711610.711610.7112.7

            Note. * = Canadian sample; FSIQ Imp. = FSIQ w/Imputed missing values imputed; FSIQ Adj. = FSIQ w/Adjustment for Flynn Effect (0.3 IQ points per year).

            Our systematic review identified only four Canadian samples among 106 samples in total, one tested with WBIS and three tested with WAIS-R. Accordingly, our main analyses include only US samples. However, we also present key meta-regression results for the full 106 US and Canadian samples as WBIS and WAIS-R did not have separate norms for Canadian population. As expected, given only four Canadian samples, the results do not change in any substantive way.

            Figure 8 shows the mean undergraduate students’ FSIQ plotted against the estimated year of testing (k = 102), for US samples only, with the size of each bubble indicating the sample size. The Figure shows a steep decline in undergraduate students’ FSIQ since the publication of the first Wechsler test, WBIS, in 1939. The figure includes a meta-regression line with 95% CI bands. The meta-regression was estimated using random effect restricted maximum likelihood estimator (“REML”option in metafor). The estimated FSIQ = 456.658 - .173 * year of testing, with corresponding R 2 = .216. The moderator test for year of testing was statistically significant, QM(df = 1) = 27.103, p < .0001. When both Canadian and US samples were included (k = 106), the estimated FSIQ = 475.431 - .183 * year of testing, with corresponding R 2 = .236. The moderator test for year of testing was statistically significant, QM(df = 1) = 31.36, p < .0001.

            Figure 8.

            A relationship between mean FSIQ and year of assessment for the US u/g samples (k = 102) without Flynn Effect adjustment. The figure includes the meta-regression line with 95% CI bands.

            Figure 9 shows the same data but with FSIQs adjusted for the Flynn Effect, for US samples only. Again, the figure shows a steep decline in undergraduate students’ FSIQ. The meta-regression was estimated using random effect restricted maximum likelihood estimator (“REML” option in metafor). The estimated FSIQ = 490.742 - .192 * year of testing with corresponding R 2 = .242. The moderator test for year of testing was statistically significant, QM(df = 1) = 31.30, p < .0001. When both Canadian and US samples were included (k = 106), the estimated FSIQ = 509.166 - .202 * year of testing, with corresponding R 2 = .261. The moderator test for year of testing was statistically significant, QM(df = 1) = 35.85, p < .0001.

            Figure 9.

            A relationship between mean FSIQ adjusted for Flynn Effect and year of assessment for the US u/g samples (k = 102). The figure includes the meta-regression line with 95% CI bands.

            Figure 10 compares the Wechsler normative samples IQ data in Table 2 with the undergraduate students’ IQs estimated from the current study. It shows FSIQs reported for WAIS normative samples with 16+ years of education and with 13–15 years of education and FSIQs adjusted for the Flynn Effect of undergraduate student samples derived from the current study. The figure highlights that, on average, undergraduate students’ FSIQs are merely average, and that the vast majority of both undergraduate students, as well as all adults with at least 16 years of education, have merely average FSIQs.

            Figure 10.

            Mean FSIQ for WAIS-R, WAIS-III, and WAIS-IV US Editions and WAIS-III CDN Edition normative samples and for US undergraduate students in the new meta-analysis (with Flynn Effect adjustment). For WAIS normative samples, mean FSIQs are shown for all examinees with 16+ years of education vs with 13–15 years of education.

            Finally, we examined the relationship between the estimated mean 2021 SAT scores (obtained from the IPEDS database) and the mean Wechsler IQ adjusted for the Flynn Effect. A simple correlation between the estimated SAT and Wechsler IQ adjusted for the Flynn Effect was moderate, r(78) = .37, p < .001. Using the estimated SAT as the 2nd moderator in addition to the year of testing revealed that the estimated SAT explained an additional 6% of the variability in the Wechsler IQs of the undergraduate samples. The estimated FSIQ = 421.280 – 0.171 * year of testing + 0.024 * SAT, with corresponding R 2 = .325. The moderator test for year of testing and SAT was statistically significant, QM(df = 2) = 37.91, p < .0001. These SAT results have to be interpreted with caution, however. The SAT data were available for only 80 out of the 106 samples, are based on only 2021 SATs of admitted students, and do not reflect the SAT of all admitted students but only those who chose to submit them.

            DISCUSSION

            The belief that on average, undergraduate students today are brilliant is a myth. In the introduction, we tracked down the origin of this myth to uncritical repetition of decades old obsolete data and claims about undergraduate students’ IQ being 115 to 130 while ignoring Flynn Effect; demonstrated that analyses of successive Wechsler normative samples revealed declines in IQ down to an average range; and reviewed massive increases in educational attainment over the last 80 years that made declines in undergraduate students IQ mathematically inevitable. Our meta-analysis provides further compelling evidence of the decline and demonstrates that the belief that, on average, undergraduate students are brilliant is a myth.

            Wechsler tests are designed to describe US and/or Canadian population, that is, the normative populations are the same but those normative populations and samples are changing as time goes by. IQ scores describe where a particular examinee or a particular group (in case of mean IQ scores) lies relative to the mean of the standardization sample (100) in terms of the standard deviation (15). Successive versions of Wechsler tests are highly correlated, indicating that they measure largely the same thing. In fact, these intercorrelations are among the highest one one can find in psychological research (0.88 to. 94), although not perfect, not 1.00 [18,23,24]. However, a wealth of research has shown that later Wechsler tests are harder than earlier tests, that the scores on one Wechsler test are not equivalent to scores on another Wechsler test, and that to compare IQ scores across successive Wechsler tests one must at minimum adjust the scores for Flynn Effect (approximately 0.3 IQ points per year).

            Our new research highlights that not only are successive Wechsler test versions harder as normative populations overall ability increases but, as compositions of normative populations change with time, performance of subgroups of normative populations also changes across successive versions of Wechsler tests. Our independent study confirms declines in mean IQs of undergraduate students reported in analyses of successive normative samples of Wechsler tests and indicate that the declines have continued for a decade and a half following norming of the WAIS-IV [24], the last Wechsler test. Today’s undergraduate students’ IQ is estimated to be mere 102 IQ points. On average, undergraduate students’ IQ is no longer extraordinary but merely average. We have also demonstrated that undergraduate students’ mean IQs vary hugely across the institutions, depending on admission standards and the selectivity of institutions the students were attending (as measured by the 2021 SAT of admitted students). The mean IQs of student samples range from below 100 to over 120, consistent with huge variability in admission rates and median SAT scores of students admitted to various universities. Even though we were using only the most recent IPEDS data on selectivity and median SAT scores of admitted students, the median SATs of admitted students moderately correlated with IQs of undergraduate students’ samples from these universities, r(78) = .37.

            The decline in undergraduate students’ mean IQs is an inevitable consequence of profound changes in educational attainment in the USA and Canada since 1939, since the publication of the WBIS [17], detailed in the introduction. Whereas only a small portion of the population of Canada and the USA ever finished high school, and only a few percent ever made it to university in 1939, almost every adult today completed high school, 60 to 70% of the population have some college or university education, and approximately 40% of adults have university degrees in USA and Canada. Accordingly, whereas the Flynn Effect describes increases in mean intelligence of successive generations corresponding to approximately 0.3 IQ points per year, our findings demonstrate that undergraduate students’ mean IQ relative to general population have been declining approximately 0.2 IQ points per year, resulting in an absolute increase of only 0.1 IQ points per year for undergraduate student population.

            Our findings have several far-reaching implications. First, professors today are no longer teaching students with mostly above-average IQs as they did in the 1950. Instead, they are teaching students with mean IQs no different from 100, that is, the mean IQs of the general population. Furthermore, professors are also teaching students with a much wider range of abilities, specifically, IQs ranging from below 70 to above 130. In the 1950s, when the average undergraduate students’ IQ was 115 to 120, only a relatively small proportion of undergraduate students had IQs below 100, whereas today, nearly half of undergraduate students have IQs below 100 – the population mean. In turn, professors have been forced to reduce material covered, reduce academic standards, reduce students’ workload, and inflate grades, degrading the value of undergraduate education [142].

            Our findings validate the views of many university professors that students are less smart, less well prepared, and work less, but yet the students themselves believe that they are, in fact, very smart and deserve the very top grades [143147]. University professors’ beliefs are also well supported in the literature. For example, students admit to studying far less than university calendars expect of them. Whereas students used to study 2–3 hours outside of the class time for each hour of class time back in 1950s, today, by their own account, students study only about one hour outside of the class time for each hour of class time [142,148,149]. Yet, if university grades reflect how smart students are, students are told by their professors that they are extraordinarily smart, smarter than students in the 1950s, since most awarded grades today are As [150,151] and, according to university calendars and grading standards, A grades are for “superior performance”, B grades are for “clearly above-average performance”, and C grades are for “satisfactory” or average performance [142]. The DFW grades (i.e., Fs, Ds, and Withdrawals) are now more rare [142]. However, as has been pointed out, the A grades given to most students do not reflect students’ superior achievement but reflect demands (a) to ensure students’ satisfaction, (b) to achieve high student evaluation of teaching (SET) ratings, (c) to minimize DFW grades, and (d) to ensure high student retention [152155]. Not surprisingly, public trust in higher education has dropped to all times low with only 36% of American public in 2023 having confidence in higher education [156].

            Second, employers can no longer expect employment applicants with undergraduate degrees to have appreciably higher IQs and mental abilities than the general population. Undergraduate students are merely average, and university graduates have, on average, a few extra IQ points but are merely average. For employers, a university degree has been losing its value and prestige for quite some time simply because there is now an abundance of individuals with such degrees. Our data also indicates that holders of university degrees are no longer special in terms of intelligence and cognitive ability as they used to be in the 1940s or 1950s. With diminishing value of undergraduate degrees, some employers allow applicants to take a quick multiple choice intelligence tests in lieu of a university degree requirement. For example, Government of Canada, one of the largest employers in Canada, allows job applicants to take General Intelligence Test GIT-310, or its newer and shorter version, General Competency Test GCT2-314, “as an alternative to a university education requirement”. To be counted as an alternative to a university education requirement, the applicant has to get 58 out of 90 multiple choice questions correct on GCT2-314 [157,158]. Many other employers have eliminated and plan to eliminate requirements for university degrees altogether [159].

            Third, students who are enrolled or who plan to enrol in higher education need to realize that acceptance into university is no longer an invitation into an elite group, that they will likely be in classes with students with huge variability in IQ ranges, and that only some portion of the education offered will be adapted to their level of ability. These students need to know that to secure many jobs that required university degrees in the past they only need to pass, for example, a 90 item multiple choice intelligence tests, specific online course, or obtain sufficient relevant experience and skills (see above).

            Fourth, various claims in scientific, clinical, and popular literature about IQs of undergraduate students and university graduates being in the above average range (detailed above), for example, between “113 and 120” [14], are plainly wrong. These claims are nothing but myths and artifacts of improper and unwarranted reliance on obsolete data sets collected decades ago, ignorance of Flynn Effect, as well as, massive change in education over the last 100 years. This misinformation ought not to be propagated by mindlessly citing decades-old articles that themselves refer to further decades-old articles and obsolete data collected in the 1940s and 1950s.

            Fifth, various methods of estimating premorbid IQs based on educational attainment are speculation and no longer evidence based as these estimates do not take into account (a) massive changes in educational attainment of populations, (b) large variability in mean IQs across institutions, (c) large variability of mean IQs across fields and subfields of study (as evidenced by SAT and GRE data detailed above), (d) large variability in IQs of individual students, and (e) Flynn Effect. For example, a clinical psychologist who opines that a client’s premorbid intelligence was clearly above average because the client (a) graduated from a Canadian public university in 2000 and (b) achieved above-average B-level grades while pursuing Bachelor’s degree in Education is clearly uninformed, ignorant of essential facts, and not minimally competent to practice in this area. First, WAIS-III Canadian Edition normative data (collected in 1996) showed that Canadians with 16 or more years of education, on average, scored in the average range with the FSIQ of 108.7 and standard deviation of 14.3 [19]. Second, students bound to pursue degrees in Education score below the average of all university-bound seniors on SAT and below the average of all students attempting GREs (see Figures 4 and 5). Third, B-grades are no longer “above-average grades” but merely average or below average grades due to a well known and widely publicized phenomenon of grade inflation [150,151]. Fourth, given the average FSIQ of 108.7 in 1996 and SD of 14.3, 95% of Canadians with 16 or more years of education had FSIQs ranging from 80 to 137. In fact, Longman et al. [19] give FSIQs of the WAIS-III normative sample for closely corresponding 2nd and 98th percentile as 78 and 142, respectively. Finally, the Flynn Effect and increases in educational attainment have continued and, as a result, the FSIQ of Canadians with 16 or more years of education was still lower in 2007, at the time WAIS-IV was normed, by another three or so IQ points, suggesting that the average WAIS-IV FSIQ of all Canadians with 16 or more years of education was only 105.7. In summary, if one wishes to speculate, the client’s IQ was likely average, around 100 or even less, rather than being above average at the time she graduated with the Bachelor’s degree in Education.

            To obtain more reasonable estimate of examinees’ premorbid IQ, clinicians need to rely on individual assessment of examinees’ IQ. First, clinicians may use SAT, ACT, GRE, and other standardized measures that are highly correlated with IQ, if such scores are available and if regression equations estimating IQ from these scores are available [36]. Second, clinicians may use various reading based and other literacy measures to estimate pre-morbid intelligence [160,161]. However, in both of these approaches, if a regression equation estimating IQ was developed for an earlier version of Wechsler test, clinicians still need to adjust the estimate for the Flynn Effect and be cognizant of the limitations of such adjustments [160].

            Sixth, education adjusted norms such as Advanced Clinical Solutions [162] norms available for WAIS-IV and Wechsler Memory Scale IV (US) are similarly mere speculations and not evidence-based for the very same reasons; the demographic adjustment for education attainment does not take into account (a) massive variability in the mean IQ of students graduating from different universities, (b) large variability of mean IQs across different fields and subfields of study, (c) large variability in IQs of individual students, (d) the Flynn effect and the resulting norms obsolescence, and (e) rapid changes in educational attainment. In fact, the use of these demographically-adjusted norms is unwarranted, wrong, and unethical; the norms attempt to adjust for the relatively small differences in IQ associated with educational attainment but ignore much larger differences in IQ between universities, fields of study, individuals, and generations.

            Finally, and critically, our research highlights what should be obvious to any informed person: obsolete IQ data ought not to be used, ever, to make high-stakes decisions about individuals, for example, by clinical psychologists, employers, vocational counsellors, or government agencies. Unfortunately, at least some psychologists, employers, vocational counsellors, and even government agencies did not yet get the message, did not read WAIS test manuals, and are unaware of trends in higher education. In particular, they appear unaware of the Flynn Effect and of rapid changes in educational attainment and education in general. For example, recently three clinical psychologists, Dr. W, S, and M, all registrants of the College of Alberta Psychologists (www.cap.ca), used Gottfredson [14] articles, Wonderlic [6] WAIS [5] IQ data, the Schmidt and Hunter [163] article that republished intelligence data on some teachers – specifically White, enlisted men in US Army Air Force at the time of World War II originally published by Harrell and Harrell [164], and the USES GATB data from 1950s [165] – to argue that an elementary school teacher, Ms. T, with twice assessed average IQ on WAIS-IV Canadian Edition [24] was so low as to be more than “2 standard deviations below the average requirement for teachers”, etc. (see Table 4 for excerpts from Dr. W’s expert report). Dr. W and S’ reports were filed as expert reports in an ongoing human rights proceedings resulting from Ms. T’s removal from the classroom in 2010 and subsequent dismissal from her employment in 2016 on the grounds that her twice assessed average intelligence and cognitive abilities prevented Ms. T from performing her teaching duties [166]. Ms. T’s employer has been explicitly relying on Dr. W and S’s opinions in an attempt to justify her removal from the classroom and the dismissal.

            Table 4:

            An extract from Dr. W’s expert report: Dr. W’s opinions about Ms. T’s intelligence based on multiple obsolete IQ norms and data sets.

            On September 21, 2021, in response to a critique of her work, Dr. W wrote in her expert report that “Data on the typical level of intelligence or general mental ability seen within a population of teachers is in fact available in the scientific literature.” and proceeded to rely on Gottfredson [4], Schmidt & Hunter [163], and Gottfredson [2] to claim that Ms. T’s twice assessed average WAIS-IV CDN [24] FSIQ was at the bottom 2% of all teachers.
            Relying on Gottfredson [4], Dr. W wrote:
            • The table below, which is extracted from a book chapter by Dr. Linda Gottfredson, shows that on average teachers’ general cognitive ability is above average, estimated at 81st percentile and equivalent to an IQ score of 113.

            Relying on Schmidt and Hunter [163], Dr. W wrote:
            • Beyond the data provided by Gottfredson, there is also empirical data about the intellectual abilities of teachers provided in a paper by Schmidt & Hunter, which is reproduced here.

            The partially reproduced Table 1 from Schmidt and Hunter [163] in Dr. W’s report indicated that 256 “Teacher[s]” had mean GCT [US Army General Classification Test] standard score of 122.8, median of 123.7, SD of 12.8, and range of 76–155. Dr. W continued:
            • The data (N = 256) shows that mean intelligence for teachers (measured with the military’s General Classification Test) was 122.8 with a standard deviation of 12.8, just below the scores for other professional occupations such as chemist, auditor, and engineer, and clearly above average.

            Relying on Gottfredson [2] figure published in and copied from Scientific American, Dr. W wrote:
            • … Note that teachers’ intellectual abilities are lumped with those of accountants and managers and clearly fall within the above average range (IQ 110–125; top 25% of the population)…

            Dr. W then opined:
            • Based on my calculations, Ms. T’s measured IQ of 86 [WAIS-IV Canadian Edition, Dr. W’s assessment, while Ms. T was physically ill, vomiting, etc., according to Dr. W’s own September 15, 2010 report; IQ of 91 WAIS-IV CDN, Dr. K’s assessment four months later] is 2 standard deviations below the average requirement for teachers.

            Dr. W, S, and M’s statements and opinions ignore that the data to which they compared Ms. T’s WAIS-IV Canadian Edition IQ scores were (a) astonishingly obsolete, (b) not representative of elementary school teachers in the USA or Canada 50 to 70 years ago nor today, and (c) collected in a historical era that had little resemblance to today. Similarly, Drs. W, S, and M never mentioned the existence of the Flynn Effect and, if one desired to speculate, the resulting need to adjust the obsolete data for 0.3 IQ points per year. In addition, they never mentioned the massive changes in educational attainment of US and Canadian populations over the last 100 years resulting in university students having merely average rather than above average mean IQ. None of the three clinical psychologists even mentioned that WAIS-III and WAIS-IV normative data already showed that university students and university graduates (individuals with 16+ years of education) had average IQs well below 110. If one wanted to speculate, adjusted for the Flynn Effect, Gottfredson’s [4] WAIS FSIQ of 112 corresponds to WAIS-IV FSIQ 96.1, and Schmidt and Hunter’s [163] CGT of 122.8 corresponds to a WAIS-IV FSIQ of 98.2. If one took the average of those two estimates, the teacher samples upon which Drs. W, S and M relied on would score, on average, a mere 97.1 on WAIS-IV. In turn, Ms. T’s WAIS-IV FSIQ scores of 86 (obtained while Ms. T was physically unwell, vomiting, being distracted by noise from adjacent room, etc) and 91 (while in more reasonable testing circumstances) are well within the centre of the distribution of these teachers as well as within the average range of WAIS-IV Canadian Edition standardization sample. These examples highlight an astonishing level of ignorance of changes that have occurred during the last 100 years, and a complete failure to examine test manuals among at least some registered clinical psychologists, including those who present themselves as experts on these matters during legal proceedings.

            Moreover, it is simply inappropriate to directly compare examinees’ IQ scores on one intelligence test to norms on some other intelligence test without some kind of equating procedures as well as recognition that estimates of examinees’ IQ scores on different test than that actually administered to them will be imprecise and subject to substantial error. Intelligence tests, including different versions of Wechsler tests, use different items, different subtests/tasks, different normative samples, and are normed at different times. As detailed above, extensive prior research indicates that even for different versions of WAIS tests, one must at minimum adjust scores or norms for the Flynn Effect. Our study highlights that as a composition of general population changes one must also adjust for the population composition changes, for example, changes in educational attainment of population and resulting decline in undergraduate students’ average IQ. Moreover, other changes in society may substantially alter performance on intelligence tests depending on specific composition of such tests. For example, an introduction of calculators and changes in school curricular de-emphasizing procedural skills and arithmetic fluency resulted substantial decline in arithmetic fluency [167]. Not surprisingly, Canadian university students in 1995 scored one half of standard deviation below the mean of Canadian General Working Population on Numerical Aptitude of General Aptitude Test Battery Canadian Edition [168] normed only ten years prior, in 1985 [169].

            Our study has several limitations. We were able to locate only four WAIS Canadian samples, and thus, were unable to examine declines in undergraduate students’ IQ in Canadian population. However, given similar massive increases in educational attainment in USA and Canada over the last 80 years, the declines in undergraduate students’ IQ in USA and Canada are likely to be comparable. If anything, we expect Canadian undergraduate students’ IQ to be slightly lower than that of US undergraduate students because Longman et al. [19] showed that associations between WAIS-III FSIQ and education attainment were much smaller in Canadian than US population (see Table 5). Thus, Canadian undergraduate students’ IQ, using Canadian norms, is likely to be only about 100 or 101 IQ points in 2022. Using Shipley-2, Uttl [170] reported that a sample of undergraduate students tested in a large undergraduate Canadian university was only 103 using Shipley-2 US norms gathered in 2008. However, if Shipley-2 was normed on Canadian population in 2022, the mean IQ of these students would be lower given the Flynn Effect, smaller association between IQ and education in Canadian population, and Canadians having slightly higher IQ scores using US vs. Canadian norms.

            Table 5:

            Mean FSIQs of WAIS normative samples with 13–15 and 16+ years of education and estimated mean FSIQs of undergraduate students at the time of Wechsler tests’ standardizations based on the current study.

            Test/Standardization YearNormative samples (US) 13–15 Years16+ YearsNormative samples (CDN) 13–15 Years16+ YearsCurrent Study (US data) UnadjustedAdjusted
            WBIS/1938121.2118.2
            WAIS/1954118.4115.1
            WAIS-R/1980107.4115.3113.9110.1
            WAIS-III/1996103.6111.6103.8108.7111.2107.0
            WAIS-IV/2007101.4107.4109.3104.9
            2022106.7102.0

            Our analyzes are limited to Wechsler adult intelligence tests only. However, Uttl [170] reported that similar declines are observed on at least two other intelligence tests: Wonderlic Personnel Test (WPT) [6] and Shipley-2 (Shipley, [171]). Wonderlic [6] reported that WPT raw scores of undergraduate students and university graduates declined substantially between 1970 to 1992 down to an average range. A recent meta-analysis of undergraduate students’ WPT scores reported in the literature confirmed these declines and showed that they continued beyond 1992 and that in 2022 undergraduate students scored on average only 22 points on WPT, corresponding to approximately 102 IQ points on IQ scale [142,169]. Similarly, Shipley (2009) reported that IQ of undergraduate students and holders of undergraduate degrees declined to average range already in 2008, 15 years ago, the time Shipley-2 was normed. Shipley (2009) wrote: “adults with less than a high school education… tended to have scores about 3 to 6 standard score points below the mean of 100 [94–97]”, “adults with a high school diploma… were found to have scores ranging from 1 to 3 points below the mean [97 to 99]”, “adults who attended some college… had scores right around the mean [99–101]” and “Individuals who had a college degree… had mean scores 3 to 7 points above the mean of 100 [103–107]” (p. 51). As detailed above, Uttl [170] reported that Canadian undergraduate students scored only 103 IQ points on Shipley-2 in 2022.

            Finally, SAT and ACT data detailed in the introduction are not comprehensive as not all students choose to submit SAT and/or ACT scores and not all students are in fact required to submit SAT and/or ACT scores. Nevertheless, SAT and ACT data are very strongly correlated and both SAT and ACT data are substantially correlated with institutional admission rates and selectivity. In turn, this suggests that both SAT and ACT data are likely representative of all admitted students.

            CONCLUSIONS

            The average IQ of undergraduate students today is a mere 102 IQ points; undergraduate students are no longer extraordinary but merely average and no different from the general population IQ (M = 100, SD = 15). From 1939 to 2022, undergraduate students’ IQ declined by approximately 0.2 IQ points per year relative to general population. The students’ average IQ also varies substantially across universities and is correlated with estimated average SAT scores of admitted students or selectivity of universities, even though the SAT and IQ data were collected at different time periods and using different samples from each institution. The decline in undergraduate students’ IQ is necessary consequence of college and university education becoming a new norm rather than the privilege of a few. In fact, graduating from university is now more common than completing high school in the 1940s or 1950s. These findings have wide-ranging implications. First, universities and professors need to realize that students are no longer extraordinary but merely average and of a wide range of abilities. Second, employers can no longer rely on job applicants with university degrees to be more capable or smarter than those without university degrees. Third, students need to realize that acceptance into university is no longer an invitation to join an elite group. Fourth, various claims in scientific, clinical and popular literature promoting the myth of extraordinarily smart undergraduate students based on obsolete data need to be promptly corrected to reflect a new reality. Fifth, various methods of estimating premorbid IQs based on educational attainment are vastly inaccurate, obsolete, no longer evidence based, and ought to be abandoned. Sixth, obsolete IQ data or tests should never be used, ever, to make high-stakes decisions about individuals by clinical psychologists, employers, vocational counsellors, or government agencies. As has been argued before, a failure to adjust obsolete test scores or norms for the Flynn Effect is unscientific, unethical, incompetent, scandalous and malpractice (see above). We agree with Reynolds et al. that “No one’s life should depend on when an IQ test was normed” and we also believe that no one’s career and livelihood should depend on the opinions of experts who opine about their clients’ job competence based on 80 years obsolete intelligence test data uncorrected for the Flynn Effect and collected in a historical era bearing little resemblance to today.

            ACKNOWLEDGEMENT

            When this study was initiated and data collected, Victoria Violo and Lacey Gibson were at Psychology Department, Mount Royal University, Calgary, Alberta, Canada. The study was funded by the Natural Science and Engineering Research Council of Canada Discovery Grant to Bob Uttl. The funder had no role in the design, data collection, analyses, or interpretation of the data.

            REFERENCES

            1. Gottfredson LS. Why g matters: the complexity of everyday life. Intelligence. 1997. Vol. 24(1):79–132. [Cross Ref]

            2. Gottfredson LS. The general intelligence factor. Sci Am. 1998. Vol. 9:24–9

            3. Gottfredson LS. Where and why g matters: not a mystery. Hum Perform. 2002. Vol. 15(1–2):25–46. [Cross Ref]

            4. Gottfredson LS. Chapter 15—G, Jobs and lifeNyborg H. The scientific study of general intelligence. Pergamon: 2003. p. 293–342. [Cross Ref]

            5. Wechsler D. Manual for the Wechsler adult intelligence scale. p. vi, 110Psychological Corp. 1955

            6. Wonderlic. Wonderlic Personnel Test and scholastic level exam user’s manual. Wonderlic Personnel Test Inc. 1992

            7. Kaufman AS, Lichtenberger EO. Assessing adolescent and adult intelligence, 3rd Edition. 3rd ed. Wiley. 2005

            8. Matarazzo JD. Wechsler’s measurement and appraisal of adult intelligence. 5th ed. Oxford University Press. 1972

            9. Jensen A. Bias in mental testing. 1st ed. Free Press. 1980

            10. Reynolds CR, Chastain RL, Kaufman AS, McLean JE. Demographic characteristics and IQ among adults: analysis of the WAIS-R standardization sample as a function of the stratification variables. J Sch Psychol. 1987. Vol. 25(4):323–42. [Cross Ref]

            11. Heaton RK, Manly JJ, Taylor MJ, Tulsky DS. Association between demographic characteristics on WAIS-III and WMS-III. 2001

            12. Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Bigler ED, Tranel D. Neuropsychological Assessment. 5th ed. Oxford University Press. 2012

            13. Anastasi A. Differential psychology. 3rd ed. Wiley. 1965

            14. Henderson R. 5 seriously stunning facts about higher education in America. Psychology Today. 2019 Accessed 30 September 2023 https://www.psychologytoday.com/us/blog/after-service/201903/5-seriously-stunning-facts-about-higher-education-in-america

            15. Cronbach LJ. Essentials of psychological testing. 2nd ed. Harper. 1960

            16. Plant WT, Richardson H. The IQ of the average college student. J Couns Psychol. 1958. Vol. 5(3):229–31. [Cross Ref]

            17. Wechsler D. The measurement of adult intelligence. Williams & Wilkins. 1939

            18. Wechsler D. Wechsler adult intelligence scale-revised. Psychological Corp. 1981

            19. Longman RS, Saklofske DH, Fung TS. WAIS-III percentile scores by education and sex for U.S. and Canadian populations. Assessment. 2007. Vol. 14(4):426–32. [Cross Ref]

            20. Fletcher JM, Stuebing KK, Hughes LC. IQ scores should be corrected for the flynn effect in high-stakes decisions. J Psychoeduc Assess. 2010. Vol. 28(5):469–73. [Cross Ref]

            21. Flynn J. The mean IQ of Americans: massive gains 1932 to 1978. Psychol Bull. 1984. Vol. 95(1):29–51

            22. Trahan L, Stuebing KK, Hiscock MK, Fletcher JM. The flynn effect: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. 2014. Vol. 140(5):1332–60. [Cross Ref]

            23. Wechsler D. Wechsler adult intelligence scale: Third Edition—technical manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. 1997

            24. Wechsler D. WAIS-IV technical and interpretive manual. Pearson. 2008

            25. Flynn J. Capital offenders and the death sentence: a scandal that must be addressed. Psychol Ment Retard Develop Disabil. 2007. Vol. 32(3):557–64

            26. Gresham FM, Reschly DJ. Standard of practice and flynn effect testimony in death penalty cases. Intellect Dev Disabil. 2011. Vol. 49(3):131–40. [Cross Ref]

            27. Reynolds CR, Niland J, Wright JE, Rosenn M. Failure to apply the flynn correction in death penalty litigation: standard practice of today maybe, but certainly malpractice of tomorrow. J Psychoeduc Assess. 2010. Vol. 28(5):477–81. [Cross Ref]

            28. US Census. Table A-1. years of school completed by people 25 years and over, by age and sex, selected years 1940 to 2021. US Census Bureau. 2022 Accessed 30 September 2023 https://www2.census.gov/programs-surveys/demo/tables/educational-attainment/time-series/cps-historical-time-series/taba-1.xlsx

            29. Chastain RL, Reynolds CR. An analysis of WAIS-R performance by sample stratification variables used during standardization. 1984 Accessed 30 September 2023 https://eric.ed.gov/?q=revised&pg=1129&id=ED249409

            30. Holdnack JA, Weiss LG. Chapter 4—Demographic adjustments to WAIS–IV/WMS–IV normsHoldnack JA, Drozdick LW, Weiss LG, Iverson GL. WAIS-IV, WMS-IV, and ACS. Academic Press. 2013. p. 171–216. [Cross Ref]

            31. College Board. 2021 SAT suite of assessment annual report total group. 2021 Accessed 30 September 2023 https://reports.collegeboard.org/media/2022-04/2021-total-group-sat-suite-of-assessments-annual-report%20%281%29.pdf

            32. College Board. SAT understanding scores 2021. 2021 Accessed 30 September 2023 https://satsuite.collegeboard.org/media/pdf/understanding-sat-scores.pdf

            33. ETS. GRE guide to the use of scores 2021-22. ETS. 2021 Accessed 30 September 2023 https://www.ets.org/gre/guide

            34. Coyle T, Pillow DR. SAT and ACT predict college GPA after removing “g”. Intelligence. 2008. Vol. 36(6):719–29

            35. Baade LE, Schoenberg MR. A proposed method to estimate premorbid intelligence utilizing group achievement measures from school records. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2004. Vol. 19(2):227–43. [Cross Ref]

            36. Collins LF. Comparative validity of NART, NART-R, AMNART, WRAT-III reading, and act scores in predicting WAIS-III FSIQS: implications for estimation of premorbid intelligence in adults (2000-95004-394; Issues 8-B). ProQuest Information & Learning. 1999 Accessed 30 September 2023 http://libproxy.mtroyal.ca/login?url=https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&AuthType=ip,url,cookie,uid&db=psyh&AN=2000-95004-394&site=ehost-live

            37. Frey MC. What we know, are still getting wrong, and have yet to learn about the relationships among the SAT, intelligence and achievement. J Intell. 2019. Vol. 7(4):26[Cross Ref]

            38. Frey MC, Detterman DK. Scholastic assessment or g? The relationship between the scholastic assessment test and general cognitive ability. Psychol Sci. 2004. Vol. 15(6):373–78. [Cross Ref]

            39. Koenig KA, Frey MC, Detterman DK. ACT and general cognitive ability. Intelligence. 2008. Vol. 36(2):153–60. [Cross Ref]

            40. Sparks RL, Lovett BJ. Objective criteria for classification of postsecondary students as learning disabled: effects on prevalence rates and group characteristics. J Learn Disabil. 2009. Vol. 42(3):230–9. [Cross Ref]

            41. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. 2021. https://www.R-project.org/

            42. Viechtbauer W. Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. J Stat Softw. 2010. Vol. 36:1–48. [Cross Ref]

            43. Aaron PG, Olsen J, Baker C. The dyslexic college student: is he also dysphasic? Cogn Psychol. 1985. Vol. 2(2):2[Cross Ref]

            44. Abell SC, Heiberger AM, Johnson JE. Cognitive evaluations of young adults by means of human figure drawings: an empirical investigation of two methods. J Clin Psychol. 1994. Vol. 50(6):6[Cross Ref]

            45. Acklin MW, Fechner-Bates S. Rorschach developmental quality and intelligence factors. J. Pers Assess. 1989. Vol. 53(3):3[Cross Ref]

            46. Advokat C, Martino L, Hill BD, Gouvier W. Continuous performance test (CPT) of college students with ADHD, psychiatric disorders, cognitive deficits, or no diagnosis. J Atten Disord. 2007. Vol. 10(3):3[Cross Ref]

            47. Allen RM, Thornton TE, Stenger CA. Ammons and Wechsler test performances of college and psychiatric subjects. J Clin Psychol. 1954. Vol. 10:378–81. [Cross Ref]

            48. Allen J, Blanton P, Johnson-Greene D, Murphy-Farmer C, Gross A. Need for achievement and performance on measures of behavioral fluency. Psychol Rep. 1992. Vol. 71(2):471–8. [Cross Ref]

            49. Anderson EE. Wilson College studies in psychology I: a comparison of the Wechsler-Bellevue, revised Stanford-Binet, and American Council on Education tests at the college level. J Psychol. 1942. Vol. 14:317–26

            50. Axelrod BN, Brines B, Rapport LJ. Estimating full scale IQ while minimizing the effects of practice. Assessment. 1997. Vol. 4(3):221–7. [Cross Ref]

            51. Bass BA, Levkulic PG. Effects of verbal reinforcements upon WAIS scores of examinees high and low in anxiety. Psychol Rep. 1985. Vol. 56(1):261–2. [Cross Ref]

            52. Beaujean AA, Knoop A, Holliday G. Does chronometry have a place in assessing math disorders? Learn Disabil Q. 2006. Vol. 29(1):32–8. [Cross Ref]

            53. Beers SR, Goldstein G, Katz LJ. Neuropsychological differences between college students with learning disabilities and those with mild head injury. J Learn Disabil. 1994. Vol. 27(5):315–24. [Cross Ref]

            54. Beglinger LJ, Smith T, Gregory RJ. Young and elderly adults’ perceptions of the Wechsler adult intelligence scale—revised. J. Clin. Psychol. 2000. Vol. 56(6):821–7. [Cross Ref]

            55. Bell NL, Lassiter KS, Matthews TD, Hutchinson MB. Comparison of the peabody picture vocabulary test—third edition and Wechsler adult intelligence scale—third edition with university students. J Clin Psychol. 2001. Vol. 57(3):417–22. [Cross Ref]

            56. Birch S, Chase C. Visual and language processing deficits in compensated and uncompensated college students with dyslexia. J Learn Disabil. 2004. Vol. 37(5):389–410. [Cross Ref]

            57. Birch SL. Prevalence and profile of phonological and surface subgroups in college students with a history of reading disability. J Learn Disabil. 2016. Vol. 49(4):339–53. [Cross Ref]

            58. Bishop EG, Dickson AL, Allen MT. Psychometric intelligence and performance on selective reminding. Clin Neuropsychol. 1990. Vol. 4(2):2[Cross Ref]

            59. Boer DP, Pugh GM. Canadian items for the WAIS--R information subtest. Can J Behav Sci/Revue Canadienne Des Sciences Du Comportement. 1988. Vol. 20(3):273–86. [Cross Ref]

            60. Buchsbaum MS, Haier RJ, Sostek AJ, Weingartner H, Zahn TP, Siever LJ, et al.. Attention dysfunction and psychopathology in college men. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1985. Vol. 42(4):354–60. [Cross Ref]

            61. Burris CL. A comparison of the Wechsler adult intelligence scale—revised with the peabody picture vocabulary test—revised. Western Kentucky University. 1983

            62. Calvin AD, Koons PB Jr, Bingham JL, Fink HH. A further investigation of the relationship between manifest anxiety and intelligence. J Consult Psychol. 1955. Vol. 19(4):280–2. [Cross Ref]

            63. Cannon R, Lubar J, Gerke A, Thornton K, Hutchens T, McCammon V. EEG spectral-power and coherence: LORETA neurofeedback training in the anterior cingulate gyrus. J Neurother. 2006. Vol. 10(1):5–31. [Cross Ref]

            64. Cannon R, Congedo M, Lubar J, Hutchens T. Differentiating a network of executive attention: LORETA neurofeedback in anterior cingulate and dorsolateral prefrontal cortices. Int J Neurosci. 2009. Vol. 119(3):404–41. [Cross Ref]

            65. Carson SH, Peterson JB, Higgins DM. Reliability, validity, and factor structure of the creative achievement questionnaire. Creat Res J. 2005. Vol. 17(1):37–50. [Cross Ref]

            66. Carvajal H, Gerber J, Hewes P, Weaver KA. Correlations between scores on stanford-binet IV and Wechsler adult intelligence scale—revised. Psychol Rep. 1987. Vol. 61(1):83–6. [Cross Ref]

            67. Carvajal H, Kixmiller J, Knapp M, Vitt J. The use of the PPST and intelligence tests in teacher education programs. Bull Psychon Soc. 1991. Vol. 29(3):192–4. [Cross Ref]

            68. Carvajal H, Schrader MS, Holmes CB. Retest reliability of the Wechsler adult intelligence scale-revised for 18- to 19-year-olds. Psychol Rep. 1996. Vol. 78(1):211–4. [Cross Ref]

            69. Clifford JS, Boufal MM, Kurtz JE. Personality traits and critical thinking skills in college students: empirical tests of a two-factor theory. Assessment. 2004. Vol. 11(2):169–76. [Cross Ref]

            70. Cole D, Weleba L. Comparison data on the Wechsler-Bellevue and the WAIS. J Clin Psychol. 1956. Vol. 12:198–9. [Cross Ref]

            71. Conry R, Plant WT. WAIS and group test prediction of an academic success criterion: high school and college. Educ Psychol Meas. 1965. Vol. 25(2):493–500. [Cross Ref]

            72. Cosden MA, McNamara J. Self-concept and perceived social support among college students with and without learning disabilities. Learn Disabil Quarterl. 1997. Vol. 20(1):2–12. [Cross Ref]

            73. Crawford MS, Boer DP. Content bias in the WAIS-R information subtest and some Canadian alternatives. Can J Behav Sci/Revue Canadienne Des Sciences Du Comportement. 1985. Vol. 17(1):79–86

            74. Davis AS, Finch WH, Drapeau C, Nogin M, Moss LE, Moore B. Predicting verbal fluency using word reading: implications for premorbid functioning. Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2016. Vol. 23(6):403–10. [Cross Ref]

            75. Dennis OC. The relative efficiency of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) as a predictor of college academic achievement. Western Kentucky University. 1978

            76. Detterman DK, Mayer JD, Caruso DR, Legree PJ, Conners FA, Taylor R. Assessment of basic cognitive abilities in relation to cognitive deficits. Am J Ment Retard. 1992. Vol. 97(3):251–86

            77. Dodd RM. The academic achievement and psychological functioning of college students with a family history of alcoholism. Ph.D., Dissertation. The University of North Dakota. 2000 Accessed 30 September 2023 https://www.proquest.com/docview/304611605/abstract/C40803F92553423BPQ/1

            78. Ducheneaux T, McDonald JD. Biculturalism and native American college students’ performance on the WAIS-III. 1999

            79. Dymond RF. Personality and empathy. J Consult Psychol. 1950. Vol. 14(5):343–50. [Cross Ref]

            80. Estes SG. Deviations of Wechsler-Bellevue subtest scores from vocabulary level in superior adults. J Abnorm Soc Psychol. 1946. Vol. 41(2):226–8. [Cross Ref]

            81. Faber K. A WAIS-IV validation study of the Altus information inventory (2021-50285-062; Issues 10-B). ProQuest Information & Learning. 2021. http://libproxy.mtroyal.ca/login?url=https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&AuthType=ip,url,cookie,uid&db=psyh&AN=2021-50285-062&site=ehost-live

            82. Feldman SE. Utility of some rapid estimations of intelligence in a college population. Psychol Rep. 1968. Vol. 22(1):23–6. [Cross Ref]

            83. Fishbein S. An evaluation of the Wechsler-Bellevue Intelligence tests for use on the college level. Unpublished M.A. thesis. Temple University. 1941

            84. Gajar A, Salvia J, Gajria M, Salvia S. A comparison of intelligence achievement discrepancies between learning disabled and non-learning disabled college students. Learn Disabil Res. 1989. Vol. 4(2):119–24

            85. Geiselman RE, MacKinnon DP, Fishman DL, Jaenicke C, Larner BR, Schoenberg S, et al.. Mechanisms of hypnotic and nonhypnotic forgetting. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 1983. Vol. 9(4):626–35. [Cross Ref]

            86. Gerbeth RA. A study of the two forms of the Wechsler-Bellevue Intelligence Scale. J Consult Psychol. 1950. Vol. 14:365–70

            87. Gooding DC, Braun JG. Visuoconstructive performance, implicit hemispatial inattention, and schizotypy. Schizophr Res. 2004. Vol. 68(2–3):261–9. [Cross Ref]

            88. Gregg N, Hoy C, Flaherty DA, Norris P, Coleman C, Davis M, et al.. Decoding and spelling accommodations for postsecondary students with dyslexia—it’s more than processing speed. Learn Disabil. 2005. Vol. 3:1–17

            89. Gregg N, Bandalos DL, Coleman C, Davis JM, Robinson K, Blake J. The validity of a battery of phonemic and orthographic awareness tasks for adults with and without dyslexia and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Remedial Spec Educ. 2008. Vol. 29(3):175–90. [Cross Ref]

            90. Hanna GS, House B, Salisbury LH. WAIS performance of Alaskan Native university freshmen. J Genet Psychol. 1968. Vol. 112(1):57–61

            91. Harrell M, Myers M, Aita S, Taylor S, Beach J, Calamia M, et al.. Mittenberg-type formula for identifying feigned ADHD using WAIS-IV. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2020. Vol. 35:1001

            92. Harwood BT. Some intellectual correlates of schizoid indicators: WAIS and MMPI. J Consult Psychol. 1967. Vol. 31(2):218[Cross Ref]

            93. Hopper BC. Examining the concurrent and predictive validity of nonverbal tests of intelligence M.A. thesis. George Fox University. 2000

            94. Ickes CS. A comparison of the WAIS—R and the MAB in a college population (1993-71299-001; Issues 12-A). ProQuest Information & Learning. 1992. http://libproxy.mtroyal.ca/login?url=https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&AuthType=ip,url,cookie,uid&db=psyh&AN=1993-71299-001&site=ehost-live

            95. Kelley MP, Coursey RD. Lateral preference and neuropsychological correlates of schizotypy. Psychiatry Res. 1992. Vol. 41(2):115–35. [Cross Ref]

            96. Kramar EJ. The relationships of the Wechsler-Bellevue and A. C. E. intelligence tests with performance scores in speaking and the Brown-Carlsen Listening Comprehension Test Ph.D. thesis. The Florida State University. 1955

            97. Ladd AH. The differential predictive value of the Wechsler-Bellevue Scale for certain area of teacher preparation D.Ed. thesis. Indiana University. 1950

            98. Lassiter KS, Bell NL, Hutchinson MB, Matthews TD. College student performance on the general ability measure for adults and the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for adults—Third Edition. Psychol Sch. 2001. Vol. 38(1):1–10. [Cross Ref]

            99. Lewis ML, Johnson JJ. Comparison of WAIS and WAIS—R IQs from two equivalent college populations. J Psychoeduc Assess. 1985. Vol. 3(1):55–60. [Cross Ref]

            100. Lott WJ. Characteristics of high standing university students on the Wechsler Bellevue Adult Intelligence Scale M.A. thesis. University of Alberta. 1952

            101. McGee S, Brown C. A split in the verbal comprehension factor in WAIS and WISC-R profiles. J Clin Psychol. 1984. Vol. 40(2):580–3. [Cross Ref]

            102. Mefferd RB. Word association: verbal intelligence. Psychol Rep. 1979. Vol. 44(3):919–24. [Cross Ref]

            103. Menary EL. A study of the relationships between reading and personality and WAIS scales at an open door community college (1986-52242-001; Issues 5-A). ProQuest Information & Learning. 1985. http://libproxy.mtroyal.ca/login?url=https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&AuthType=ip,url,cookie,uid&db=psyh&AN=1986-52242-001&site=ehost-live

            104. Merrill RM, Heathers LB. Centile scores for the Wechsler-Bellevue Intelligence Scale on a university counseling center group. J Consult Psychol. 1952. Vol. 16(5):406–9. [Cross Ref]

            105. Merrill RM, Heathers LB. A comparison of the Wechsler-Bellevue and ACE tests on a university counseling center group. J Consult Psychol. 1953. Vol. 17(1):63–6. [Cross Ref]

            106. Mishra SP, Brown KH. The comparability of WAIS and WAIS-R IQs and subset scores. J Clin Psychol. Vol. 39(5):754–7. [Cross Ref]

            107. Morgan AW, Sullivan SA, Darden C, Gregg N. Measuring the intelligence of college students with learning disabilities: a comparison of results obtained on the WAIS-R and the KAIT. J Learn Disabil. 1997. Vol. 30(5):560–5. [Cross Ref]

            108. Morris-Friehe M, Leuenberger J. Direct and indirect measures of writing for nonlearning disabled and learning disabled college students. Read Writ. 1992. Vol. 4(3):281–96. [Cross Ref]

            109. Mosberg L, Johns D. Reading and listening comprehension in college students with developmental dyslexia. Learn Disabil Res Pract. 1994. Vol. 9(3):130–5

            110. Nobo J, Evans RG. The WAIS—R picture arrangement and comprehension subtests as measures of social behavior characteristics. J Pers Assess. 1986. Vol. 50(1):90–2

            111. O’Hora D, Peláez M, Barnes-Holmes D, Rae G, Robinson K, Chaudhary T. Temporal relations and intelligence: correlating relational performance with performance on the WAIS-III. Psychol Rec. 2008. Vol. 58(4):569–84

            112. Olsen IA, Jordheim GD. Use of WAIS in a student counseling center. Pers Guid J. 1964. Vol. 42(5):500–3. [Cross Ref]

            113. Ormrod JE. Comparing good and poor spellers of equal reading and verbal abilities. Percept Mot Skills. 1990. Vol. 71(2):432–4. [Cross Ref]

            114. Paul SM. The Advanced Raven’s Progressive Matrices: normative data for an American university population and an examination of the relationship with Spearman’s g. J Exp Educ. 1985. Vol. 54(2):95–100. [Cross Ref]

            115. Pilgrim BM. Examination of three short forms for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (2001-95002-144; Issues 7-B). ProQuest Information & Learning. 2001

            116. Plant WT. Mental ability scores for freshmen in a California state college. Calif J Educ Res. 1958. Vol. 9:72–3

            117. Quereshi MY, Ostrowski MJ. The comparability of three Wechsler adult intelligence scales in a college sample. J Clin Psychol. 1985. Vol. 41(3):397–407. [Cross Ref]

            118. Rakusin JM. The analysis of scatter on the Wechsler-Bellevue Adult Intelligence Scale in a group of adjusted and a group of maladjusted college students Unpublished M.S. thesis. Pennsylvania State College. 1949

            119. Ratcliff R, Thapar A, McKoon G. Individual differences, aging, and IQ in two-choice tasks. Cogn Psychol. 2010. Vol. 60(3):127–57. [Cross Ref]

            120. Rossini ED, Wygonik EJ, Barrett DE, Friedman B. WAIS—R validation of the thurstone test of mental alertness. Psychol Rep. 1994. Vol. 74 3_suppl:1339–45. [Cross Ref]

            121. Ruble VE. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale as a predictor of college success with high risk students (1982-50117-001; Issues 8-A). ProQuest Information & Learning. 1981. http://libproxy.mtroyal.ca/login?url=https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&AuthType=ip,url,cookie,uid&db=psyh&AN=1982-50117-001&site=ehost-live

            122. Salvia SA, Salvia J. Significant Discrepancies between the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale--revised and the woodcock-johnson psycho-educational battery, part II: tests of achievement with a college population. Diagnostique. 1986. Vol. 11(2):59–68

            123. Salvia J, Gajar A, Gajria M, Salvia S. A comparison of WAIS—R profiles of nondisabled college freshmen and college students with learning disabilities. J Learn Disabil. 1988. Vol. 21(10):632–6. [Cross Ref]

            124. Sartain AQ. A comparison of the new Revised Stanford-Binet, the Bellevue Scale, and certain group tests of intelligence. J Soc Psychol. 1946. Vol. 23:237–59

            125. Sedlacek GM. The Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale and the Washington Pre-College Test battery as predictors of academic success (1978-11220-001; Issues 6-A). ProQuest Information & Learning. 1976. http://libproxy.mtroyal.ca/login?url=https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&AuthType=ip,url,cookie,uid&db=psyh&AN=1978-11220-001&site=ehost-live

            126. Shaw DJ. Sexual bias in the WAIS. J Consult Psychol. 1965. Vol. 29(6):590–1. [Cross Ref]

            127. Sheckart GR, Bass BA. The effects of verbal and nonverbal contingent reinforcement upon the intelligence test performance of Black adults. J Clin Psychol. 1976. Vol. 32(4):826–8. [Cross Ref]

            128. Sheldon MS, Coale JM, Copple R. Concurrent validity of the “warm teacher scale”. J Educ Psychol. 1959. Vol. 50(1):37–40. [Cross Ref]

            129. Small MA, Raney JF, Knapp TJ. Complex reaction time and general intelligence: a refinement. J Genetic Psychol. 1987. Vol. 148(4):405–14. [Cross Ref]

            130. Smith RS. A comparison study of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale and the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised in a college population. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1983. Vol. 51(3):414–9. [Cross Ref]

            131. Sorensen MA, Clifton SW. Client opinions of WAIS interpretations. Meas Eval Guidance. 1968. Vol. 1(3):3

            132. Steisel IM. The relation between test and retest scores on the Wechsler-Bellevue Scale (Form I) for selected college students. J Genet Psychol. 1951. Vol. 79:155–62. [Cross Ref]

            133. Storrs SV. Evaluative data on the G. A. T. B. Pers Guid J. 1952. Vol. 31:87–90. [Cross Ref]

            134. Thompson A, Plumridge S. Two- and four-subtest short forms of the WAIS-R: a comparative validity study with a normal sample. Psychol Rep. 1999. Vol. 84(2):371–80. [Cross Ref]

            135. Titus JB, Retzlaff PD, Dean RS. Predicting scores of the Halstead Category Test with the WAIS-III. Int J Neurosci. 2002. Vol. 112(9):1099–114. [Cross Ref]

            136. Verney SP, Granholm E, Marshall SP, Malcarne VL, Saccuzzo DP. Culture-fair cognitive ability assessment: information processing and psychophysiological approaches. Assessment. 2005. Vol. 12(3):3[Cross Ref]

            137. Wall HW, Marks EF, Ford DH, Zeigler ML. Estimates of the concurrent validity of the WAIS and normative distributions for college freshment. Personnel Guid J. 1962. Vol. 40:717–722

            138. Ward TB, Stagner BH, Scott JG, Marcus-Mendoza ST, Turner D. Classification behavior and measures of intelligence: dimensional identity versus overall similarity. Percept Psychophys. 1989. Vol. 45(1):1[Cross Ref]

            139. Weyandt LL, Mitzlaff L, Thomas L. The relationship between intelligence and performance on the Test of Variables of Attention (TOVA). J Learn Disabil. 2002. Vol. 35(2):2[Cross Ref]

            140. Whitworth RH, Gibbons RT. Cross-racial comparison of the WAIS and WAIS—R. Educ Psychol Meas. 1986. Vol. 46(4):4[Cross Ref]

            141. Young SR, Keith TZ. An examination of the convergent validity of the ICAR16 and WAIS-IV. J. Psychoeduc Assess. 2020. Vol. 38(8):8[Cross Ref]

            142. Uttl B. Student Evaluation of Teaching (SET): Why the emperor has no clothes and what we should do about it. Human Arenas. 2023. [Cross Ref]

            143. CTV.ca News Staff. Profs say students lack maturity, feel entitled | CTV News. 2009. https://www.ctvnews.ca/profs-say-students-lack-maturity-feel-entitled-1.386965

            144. Douglas SM. Mind over merit. Education Forum (Toronto. 1988). 2009. Vol. 35(3):11

            145. Frank N. The increase in narcissism in college students. Owlcation. 2022. https://owlcation.com/social-sciences/Increasing-Narcissism-in-College-Students

            146. Greenberger E, Lessard J, Chen C, Farruggia SP. Self-entitled college students: contributions of personality, parenting, and motivational factors. J Youth Adolescence. 2008. Vol. 37(10):1193–204. [Cross Ref]

            147. Keener A. An examination of psychological characteristics and their relationship to academic entitlement among millennial and nonmillennial college students. Psychol Sch. 2020. Vol. 57(4):572–82. [Cross Ref]

            148. Babcock PS, Marks M. The falling time cost of college: evidence from half a century of time use data (Working Paper 15954; Working Paper Series, Issue 15954). National Bureau of Economic Research. 2010. [Cross Ref]

            149. Fosnacht K, McCormick AC, Lerma R. First-year students’ time use in college: a latent profile analysis. Res High Educ. 2018. Vol. 59(7):958–78. [Cross Ref]

            150. Rojstaczer S, Healy C. Grading in American colleges and universities. Teach Coll Rec. 2010.

            151. Rojstaczer S, Healy C. Where a is ordinary: the evolution of American College and University Grading, 1940-2009. Teach Coll Rec. 2012. Vol. 114(7):1–23

            152. Stroebe W. Why good teaching evaluations may reward bad teaching: on grade inflation and other unintended consequences of student evaluations. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2016. Vol. 11(6):800–16. [Cross Ref]

            153. Stroebe W. Student evaluations of teaching encourages poor teaching and contributes to grade inflation: a theoretical and empirical analysis. Basic Appl Soc Psych. 2020. Vol. 42(4):276–94. [Cross Ref]

            154. Uttl B. Lessons learned from research on student evaluation of teaching in higher educationRollett W, Bijlsma H, Röhl S. Student feedback on teaching in schools: using student perceptions for the development of teaching and teachers. Springer International Publishing. 2021. p. 237–56. [Cross Ref]

            155. Uttl B, White CA, Gonzalez DW. Meta-analysis of faculty’s teaching effectiveness: student evaluation of teaching ratings and student learning are not related. Stud Educ Eval. 2017. Vol. 54:22–42. [Cross Ref]

            156. Schermele Z. Public trust in higher Ed has plummeted. Yes, again. 2023. https://www.chronicle.com/article/public-trust-in-higher-ed-has-plummeted-yes-again

            157. Government of Canada. General competency test level 2 (GCT2-314). Government of Canada. 2024 Accessed 6 February 2024 https://www.canada.ca/en/public-service-commission/services/staffing-assessment-tools-resources/human-resources-specialists-hiring-managers/human-resources-toolbox/personnel-psychology-centre/consultation-test-services/public-service-commission-tests/general-competency-test-level-2-gct2.html

            158. Government of Canada. General intelligence test (GIT-310). Government of Canada. 2024 Accessed 6 February 2024 https://www.canada.ca/en/public-service-commission/services/staffing-assessment-tools-resources/human-resources-specialists-hiring-managers/human-resources-toolbox/personnel-psychology-centre/consultation-test-services/public-service-commission-tests/general-intelligence-test-310-git-310.html

            159. Desai NG. How the college degree lost its value: nearly half of US companies plan to ax Bachelor’s degree requirements—after walmart, accenture and IBM led the charge. Daily Mail. 2023. Accessed 6 February 2024 https://www.dailymail.co.uk/yourmoney/consumer/article-12806053/companies-ax-college-bachelors-degree-requirements-walmart.html

            160. Kirton JW, Soble JR, Marceaux JC, Messerly J, Bain KM, Webber TA, et al.. Comparison of models of premorbid IQ estimation using the TOPF, OPIE-3, and barona equation, with corrections for the flynn effect. Neuropsychology. 2020. Vol. 34(1):43–52. [Cross Ref]

            161. Manly JJ, Byrd D, Touradji P, Sanchez D, Stern Y. Literacy and cognitive change among ethnically diverse elders. Int J Psychol. 2004. Vol. 39(1):47–60. [Cross Ref]

            162. Wechsler D. Advanced clinical solutions for WAIS-IV and WMS-IV. Pearson. 2009

            163. Schmidt FL, Hunter J. General mental ability in the world of work: occupational attainment and job performance. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2004. Vol. 86(1):162–73. [Cross Ref]

            164. Harrell TW, Harrell MS. Army general classification test scores for civilian occupations. Educ Psychol Meas. 1945. Vol. 5:229–39. [Cross Ref]

            165. US DOL. Manual for the USES general aptitude test battery section III: development. Manpower Administration Department of Labor. 1970

            166. Uttl B. Pronouncing opinions about clients based on obsolete data sets: Minimally competent practice, unprofessional conduct, and/or malpractice? [Conversation Session/Ethics Stream]. Toronto, ON: Canadian Psychological Association. 2023 Accessed 6 February 2024 https://sd5bc.info/wp-content/uploads/2023/08/Uttl-CPA-2023-EthicsObsoleteTests.pdf

            167. LeFevre J-A, Penner-Wilger M, Pyke AA, Shanahan T, Deslauriers WA. Putting two and two together: declines in arithmetic fluency among young Canadian adults, 1993 to 2005 (Carleton University Cognitive Science Technical Report 2014–01). Carleton University. 2014

            168. Nelson. Manual for the general aptitude test battery Canadian 1986 edition. Nelson, Canada: 1986

            169. Yeasting KC. Evaluation of the general aptitude test battery-computerized administration (GATB-CA). ProQuest Dissertations Publishing. 1996 Accessed 6 February 2024 https://search.proquest.com/docview/304336731

            170. Uttl B. Average intelligence of university students is merely average and implications are wide-ranging [Talk]. Toronto, ON: Canadian Psychological Association. 2023

            171. Shipley WC, Gruber CP, Martin TA, Klein AM. Shipley-2 Manual. Western Psychological Services. Torrance, CA:

            Author and article information

            Journal
            sor
            ScienceOpen Research
            ScienceOpen
            2199-1006
            01 May 2024
            : e20240002
            Affiliations
            [1 ] Psychology Department, Mount Royal University, Calgary, Alberta, Canada ( https://ror.org/04evsam41)
            [2 ] Psychology Department, University of British Columbia Okanagan, Kelowna, British Columbia, Canada ( https://ror.org/03rmrcq20)
            [3 ] Psychology Department, Western University, London, Ontario, Canada ( https://ror.org/02grkyz14)
            Author notes
            *Corresponding author’s e-mail address: uttlbob@ 123456gmail.com
            Author information
            https://orcid.org/0000-0002-9908-9222
            Article
            S2199-1006.1.SOR.2024.0002.v1
            10.14293/S2199-1006.1.SOR.2024.0002.v1
            c60ec2c6-dada-4dbb-b861-7f10730079a2
            Copyright © 2024 Uttl B, Violo V, Gibson L.

            This work has been published open access under Creative Commons Attribution License CC BY 4.0 https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. Conditions, terms of use and publishing policy can be found at www.scienceopen.com.

            History
            Page count
            Figures: 10, Tables: 5, References: 171, Pages: 24

            demographic adjustments,Wechsler Adult Intelligence Test,intelligence,IQ,undergraduate students,Flynn Effect,high-stakes decisions

            Comments

            Comment on this article