3
views
0
recommends
+1 Recommend
0 collections
    0
    shares
      • Record: found
      • Abstract: found
      • Article: found
      Is Open Access

      Fallacies in Neuroscience: The Alzheimer's Edition

      article-commentary
      eNeuro
      Society for Neuroscience
      Alzheimer’s disease, amyloid cascade hypothesis, β-amyloid, clinical trials, neurofibrillary tangles

      Read this article at

      Bookmark
          There is no author summary for this article yet. Authors can add summaries to their articles on ScienceOpen to make them more accessible to a non-specialist audience.

          Abstract

          In his editorial entitled “On Fallacies in Neuroscience,” Bernard (2020) takes his fellow neuroscientists to task for failures in logic that lead to the promulgation of fallacies and their entrenchment in the scientific literature. Drawing on his background in mathematics and philosophy, he highlights two common errors in logic that permeate our field. The first error he discusses leads to a fallacy known as “reverse inference.” The example he cites is subtle, but all the more telling because it “feels” correct before we are shown the problem. Imagine that my brain is in a scanner when I experience a bout of fear. The scan shows that a particular brain region (Bernard calls it Area Z) lights up brightly with the onset of my fear. One would be correct in proposing that Area Z is likely involved in my brain's processing of fear. The fallacious conclusion, however, would be to assert that if Area Z is lit, I must be experiencing fear. That is the “reverse” of the experimental design, which was to induce fear and see which brain region activates. As Bernard (2020) points out, one could easily imagine that if I were experiencing a different emotion, even joy, Area Z might also light up. Without a lot more data, all we know is that the region is correlated with my experience of fear. We do not know that it is either necessary or sufficient to cause my fear. The second error he describes leads to a fallacy known as “affirming the consequent.” His choice of example in this case is far easier to grasp. He starts with two objectively true statements. The first one is a conditional statement: if Francis Bacon wrote Hamlet, then he had to have been a great writer. The second is a simple, true observation: Francis Bacon was a great writer. Bernard then cautions us to avoid what is in this case an obvious logical fallacy: from these two facts alone, we cannot conclude that Francis Bacon wrote Hamlet. As I read Bernard's short celebration of the importance of logical thinking, I was struck at how completely the field of Alzheimer's disease research has fallen into both fallacy traps he describes. The error of reverse inference can be seen in the very foundation of the amyloid cascade hypothesis. We know that people with Alzheimer's disease have plaques and tangles in their brains. Plaques and tangles are associated with Alzheimer’s disease. However, just as we cannot conclude that Area Z causes fear, we are not entitled to conclude that plaques and tangles cause Alzheimer’s disease. It is OK to use these data to form and state a hypothesis: plaques and tangles might cause Alzheimer’s disease. Indeed, that is precisely what Hardy and Higgins (1992) did in their first exposition of the amyloid cascade hypothesis. The trap that the field has fallen into is to use reverse inference to turn this hypothesis into dogma. The second fallacy is equally prevalent in the field. Rather than Bacon wrote Hamlet, we instead have very wise people telling us that amyloid and tau cause Alzheimer’s disease. This is supported, largely, by the same logic that was used above to deny Shakespeare credit for writing Hamlet. We start with two objectively true statements. First, the conditional: if plaques and tangles cause Alzheimer’s disease, then their constituents [aggregated β-amyloid (Aβ) and/or tau] must be neurotoxic. The second is a short, objectively true observation: Aβ and tau are neurotoxic. These two facts are consistent with the hypothesis that plaques and tangles cause Alzheimer’s disease, but they are not proof. As I detail in my new book (Herrup, 2021), we have acquired a great deal of data since the first exposition of the idea of an amyloid cascade, and most of it tends to falsify the original hypothesis. Yet, it persists in the literature, indeed dominates it, not as a hypothesis but as dogma. As I have argued before (Herrup, 2015), the field has tested two of its most basic predictions and they have failed. We have added tau and amyloid to the brains of healthy mice and humans, yet the predicted onslaught of Alzheimer’s disease either does not occur or is so slow to appear that common sense would have us search for other, perhaps more direct causes. We have also removed tau and amyloid from the brains of people with Alzheimer’s disease, yet they continue their cognitive decline (Mullard, 2021a, b; Reiss et al., 2021). We still debate whether the decline is less steep than in untreated persons, but under even the most optimistic readings of the data, the effect of removing amyloid or tau is modest. To these two fallacies, I would add a third: the assumption of causality from an observed temporal sequence. As I do not have a background in philosophy, I do not know whether there is a formal name for this error. The fallacy is best exemplified by the famous diagram shown in Figure 1. Listing the S-shaped curves from left to right provides the following sequence: Aβ in CSF precedes PET-detectable amyloid deposits, which precede tau in CSF, which precedes reductions in brain activity or volume, which precede cognitive impairment. This list is true (although the shapes of the curves are somewhat speculative). But just because Aβ comes before tau in time does not mean that Aβ causes the changes in tau. That can be seen simply by my modified version of the figure (Fig. 2). I am being ridiculous to make the logical fallacy clear. My revised sequence of events is objectively true. Yet no one would argue that gray hair causes the rise in CSF Aβ or any of the other changes in the diagram. Figure 1. Average rates of appearance of amyloid and tau markers in Alzheimer's disease (after Jack et al., 2013). Figure 2. Modification of Figure 1. Stated as a hypothesis of the importance of amyloid in the pathogenesis of Alzheimer’s disease, Figure 1 is as useful as the original statement of the amyloid cascade hypothesis. But it is only useful in a seminar or publication if it is clearly identified as a hypothesis. To treat it as fact is the same error as arranging a line of 20 random stones in order of increasing size, then using a photograph of the arrangement to prove that stones grow. The question before us then is what can be done to reduce the burden that these fallacies place on the field; what is our path forward? I must admit that I am rather optimistic about our prospects for finding new Alzheimer’s disease treatments. Despite a string of new publications arguing for the continued value of the amyloid cascade hypothesis (for review, see Selkoe, 2021), novel and exciting nonamyloid alternatives are increasingly being developed and tested, even as anti-tau and anti-amyloid treatments remain a significant part of our clinical trial portfolio. That said, I cannot shake a deep pessimism from what I perceive as the near futility of hoping that our field will ever honestly address the logical weaknesses of applying the amyloid cascade hypothesis to the pathogenesis of Alzheimer’s disease. I am reminded of the old adage that holds that it is hard to make a person understand something if their salary depends on their not understanding it. For the pharmaceutical industry, the “salary” is the deep financial investments that they have made in therapeutic approaches based on the amyloid cascade. For the National Institutes of Health and many of the private charities that fund Alzheimer’s disease research, the salary is not only their financial commitments, but also their international reputation. These groups have repeatedly told investors, politicians, and donors that they know how to get this done. If amyloid is not the cause of Alzheimer’s disease, there will be a lot of very uncomfortable explaining to be done. There are arguments that can be made for continuing to pursue anti-amyloid and anti-tau approaches: “we just need to start earlier in the disease process,” or “we just need to pick our trial population more carefully,” or “we just need to design our trials more efficiently,” etc. What is clear, however, is that our field lacks the courage to answer one simple question: what experimental result could we agree on that would falsify the amyloid cascade hypothesis? If the answer to that question is “none,” then it is not a hypothesis and has no logical value. In thus abandoning logic and clinging to dogma, my fear is that Alzheimer’s disease research will be mired in fallacies for some time to come. And the costs—in lost time, lost money, and lost lives—will be enormous.

          Related collections

          Most cited references9

          • Record: found
          • Abstract: found
          • Article: not found

          Tracking pathophysiological processes in Alzheimer's disease: an updated hypothetical model of dynamic biomarkers.

          In 2010, we put forward a hypothetical model of the major biomarkers of Alzheimer's disease (AD). The model was received with interest because we described the temporal evolution of AD biomarkers in relation to each other and to the onset and progression of clinical symptoms. Since then, evidence has accumulated that supports the major assumptions of this model. Evidence has also appeared that challenges some of our assumptions, which has allowed us to modify our original model. Refinements to our model include indexing of individuals by time rather than clinical symptom severity; incorporation of interindividual variability in cognitive impairment associated with progression of AD pathophysiology; modifications of the specific temporal ordering of some biomarkers; and recognition that the two major proteinopathies underlying AD biomarker changes, amyloid β (Aβ) and tau, might be initiated independently in sporadic AD, in which we hypothesise that an incident Aβ pathophysiology can accelerate antecedent limbic and brainstem tauopathy. Copyright © 2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
            Bookmark
            • Record: found
            • Abstract: not found
            • Article: not found

            Alzheimer's disease: the amyloid cascade hypothesis

              Bookmark
              • Record: found
              • Abstract: found
              • Article: not found

              The case for rejecting the amyloid cascade hypothesis.

              Alzheimer's disease (AD) is a biologically complex neurodegenerative dementia. Nearly 20 years ago, with the combination of observations from biochemistry, neuropathology and genetics, a compelling hypothesis known as the amyloid cascade hypothesis was formulated. The core of this hypothesis is that it is pathological accumulations of amyloid-β, a peptide fragment of a membrane protein called amyloid precursor protein, that act as the root cause of AD and initiate its pathogenesis. Yet, with the passage of time, growing amounts of data have accumulated that are inconsistent with the basically linear structure of this hypothesis. And while there is fear in the field over the consequences of rejecting it outright, clinging to an inaccurate disease model is the option we should fear most. This Perspective explores the proposition that we are over-reliant on amyloid to define and diagnose AD and that the time has come to face our fears and reject the amyloid cascade hypothesis.
                Bookmark

                Author and article information

                Journal
                eNeuro
                eNeuro
                eneuro
                eNeuro
                eNeuro
                Society for Neuroscience
                2373-2822
                10 February 2022
                10 February 2022
                Jan-Feb 2022
                : 9
                : 1
                : ENEURO.0530-21.2021
                Affiliations
                Department of Neurobiology, School of Medicine, University of Pittsburgh , Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania 15213-2548
                Author notes

                The author declares no competing financial interests.

                Author contributions: K.H. wrote the paper.

                This work was supported by the Department of Neurobiology, School of Medicine, University of Pittsburgh.

                Correspondence should be sent to Karl Herrup at herrup@ 123456pitt.edu .
                Author information
                https://orcid.org/0000-0001-7786-5844
                Article
                eN-COM-0530-21
                10.1523/ENEURO.0530-21.2021
                8856698
                35144999
                99d29946-c2ed-48f1-8ac0-430153a517bf
                Copyright © 2022 Herrup

                This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.

                History
                : 23 December 2021
                : 24 December 2021
                Page count
                Figures: 2, Tables: 0, Equations: 0, References: 9, Pages: 3, Words: 00
                Funding
                Funded by: Dept of Neurobiology, University of Pittsburgh
                Categories
                0625
                3
                Commentary
                Disorders of the Nervous System
                Custom metadata
                January/February 2022

                alzheimer’s disease,amyloid cascade hypothesis,β-amyloid,clinical trials,neurofibrillary tangles

                Comments

                Comment on this article