1
views
0
recommends
+1 Recommend
0 collections
    0
    shares
      • Record: found
      • Abstract: found
      • Article: found
      Is Open Access

      The role of kinship and demography in shaping cooperation amongst male lions

      research-article

      Read this article at

      Bookmark
          There is no author summary for this article yet. Authors can add summaries to their articles on ScienceOpen to make them more accessible to a non-specialist audience.

          Abstract

          The influence of kinship on animal cooperation is often unclear. Cooperating Asiatic lion coalitions are linearly hierarchical; male partners appropriate resources disproportionately. To investigate how kinship affect coalitionary dynamics, we combined microsatellite based genetic inferences with long-term genealogical records to measure relatedness between coalition partners of free-ranging lions in Gir, India. Large coalitions had higher likelihood of having sibling partners, while pairs were primarily unrelated. Fitness computations incorporating genetic relatedness revealed that low-ranking males in large coalitions were typically related to the dominant males and had fitness indices higher than single males, contrary to the previous understanding of this system based on indices derived from behavioural metrics alone. This demonstrates the indirect benefits to (related) males in large coalitions. Dominant males were found to ‘lose less’ if they lost mating opportunities to related partners versus unrelated males. From observations on territorial conflicts we show that while unrelated males cooperate, kin-selected benefits are ultimately essential for the maintenance of large coalitions. Although large coalitions maximised fitness as a group, demographic parameters limited their prevalence by restricting kin availability. Such demographic and behavioural constraints condition two-male coalitions to be the most attainable compromise for Gir lions.

          Related collections

          Most cited references51

          • Record: found
          • Abstract: not found
          • Article: not found

          The genetical evolution of social behaviour. I.

            Bookmark
            • Record: found
            • Abstract: found
            • Article: not found

            Rapid and simple method for purification of nucleic acids.

            We have developed a simple, rapid, and reliable protocol for the small-scale purification of DNA and RNA from, e.g., human serum and urine. The method is based on the lysing and nuclease-inactivating properties of the chaotropic agent guanidinium thiocyanate together with the nucleic acid-binding properties of silica particles or diatoms in the presence of this agent. By using size-fractionated silica particles, nucleic acids (covalently closed circular, relaxed circular, and linear double-stranded DNA; single-stranded DNA; and rRNA) could be purified from 12 different specimens in less than 1 h and were recovered in the initial reaction vessel. Purified DNA (although significantly sheared) was a good substrate for restriction endonucleases and DNA ligase and was recovered with high yields (usually over 50%) from the picogram to the microgram level. Copurified rRNA was recovered almost undegraded. Substituting size-fractionated silica particles for diatoms (the fossilized cell walls of unicellular algae) allowed for the purification of microgram amounts of genomic DNA, plasmid DNA, and rRNA from cell-rich sources, as exemplified for pathogenic gram-negative bacteria. In this paper, we show representative experiments illustrating some characteristics of the procedure which may have wide application in clinical microbiology.
              Bookmark
              • Record: found
              • Abstract: found
              • Article: not found

              Co-residence patterns in hunter-gatherer societies show unique human social structure.

              Contemporary humans exhibit spectacular biological success derived from cumulative culture and cooperation. The origins of these traits may be related to our ancestral group structure. Because humans lived as foragers for 95% of our species' history, we analyzed co-residence patterns among 32 present-day foraging societies (total n = 5067 individuals, mean experienced band size = 28.2 adults). We found that hunter-gatherers display a unique social structure where (i) either sex may disperse or remain in their natal group, (ii) adult brothers and sisters often co-reside, and (iii) most individuals in residential groups are genetically unrelated. These patterns produce large interaction networks of unrelated adults and suggest that inclusive fitness cannot explain extensive cooperation in hunter-gatherer bands. However, large social networks may help to explain why humans evolved capacities for social learning that resulted in cumulative culture.
                Bookmark

                Author and article information

                Contributors
                stotra@umn.edu
                jhalay@wii.gov.in
                Journal
                Sci Rep
                Sci Rep
                Scientific Reports
                Nature Publishing Group UK (London )
                2045-2322
                16 October 2020
                16 October 2020
                2020
                : 10
                : 17527
                Affiliations
                [1 ]GRID grid.17635.36, ISNI 0000000419368657, Department of Fisheries, Wildlife & Conservation Biology, University of Minnesota, ; 2003 Buford Circle, 150 Skok Hall, St. Paul, MN 55108 USA
                [2 ]GRID grid.452923.b, ISNI 0000 0004 1767 4167, Department of Animal Ecology & Conservation Biology, , Wildlife Institute of India, ; Chandrabani, Dehradun, Uttarakhand 248 001 India
                Article
                74247
                10.1038/s41598-020-74247-x
                7568578
                33067511
                da5180cf-2220-4085-9d70-a338769d1614
                © The Author(s) 2020

                Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

                History
                : 27 June 2020
                : 24 September 2020
                Funding
                Funded by: FundRef http://dx.doi.org/10.13039/501100001409, Department of Science and Technology, Ministry of Science and Technology, India;
                Award ID: SERB/F/0601/2013-2016
                Funded by: University of Minnesota
                Funded by: Wildlife Institute of India, India
                Categories
                Article
                Custom metadata
                © The Author(s) 2020

                Uncategorized
                behavioural ecology,ecological genetics,evolutionary ecology
                Uncategorized
                behavioural ecology, ecological genetics, evolutionary ecology

                Comments

                Comment on this article